Skip to main content
Log in

Early Developments in Joint Action

  • Joint Action: What is Shared?
  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Joint action, critical to human social interaction and communication, has garnered increasing scholarly attention in many areas of inquiry, yet its development remains little explored. This paper reviews research on the growth of joint action over the first 2 years of life to show how children become progressively more able to engage deliberately, autonomously, and flexibly in joint action with adults and peers. It is suggested that a key mechanism underlying the dramatic changes in joint action over the second year of life is the ability to reflect consciously on oneself and one’s behavior and volition and correspondingly, on the behavior, goals, and intentions of others.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adamson, L., and J. Frick. 2003. The still face: A history of a shared experimental paradigm. Infancy 4(4): 451–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amsterdam, B. 1972. Mirror self-image reactions before age two. Developmental Psychobiology 5: 297–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I.A., and S.A. Butterfill. 2009. Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review 116(4): 953–970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asendorpf, J.B., and P.M. Baudonniere. 1993. Self-awareness and other-awareness: Mirror self-recognition and synchronic imitation among unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology 29(1): 88–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashley, J., and M. Tomasello. 1998. Cooperative problem-solving and teaching in preschoolers. Social Development 7(2): 143–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aureli, T., and F. Presaghi. 2010. Developmental trajectories for mother-infant coregulation in the second year of life. Infancy 15(6): 557–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., and L.B. Adamson. 1984. Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction. Child Development 55(4): 1278–1289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., and L.B. Adamson. 1986. Infants’ conventionalized acts: Gestures and words with mothers and peers. Infant Behavior & Development 9: 215–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barresi, J., and C. Moore. 1996. Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19(1): 107–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., L. Camaioni, and V. Volterra. 1975. The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 21: 205–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bigelow, A., and L. Walden. 2009. Infants’ response to maternal mirroring in the still face and replay tasks. Infancy 14(5): 526–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M., and K. Tamis-LeMonda. 2001. Mother-infant interaction. In Infant Development, ed. G. Bremner and A. Fogel, 269–295. Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bratman, M.E. 1992. Shared cooperative activity. Philosophical Review 101(2): 327–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bratman, M.E. 1993. Shared Intention. Ethics 104(1): 97–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks Gunn, J., and M. Lewis. 1984. The development of early visual self-recognition. Developmental Review 4(3): 215–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, C., and E. Brown. 1992. Peers and play in infants and toddlers. In Handbook of social development: A lifespan perspective, ed. V. Van Hasselt and M. Hersen, 183–200. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, C., and M. Carriger. 1990. Changes in cooperation and self-other differentiation during the second year. Child Development 61: 1164–1174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, C., G. Ramani, and S. Zerwas. 2006. Becoming a social partner with peers: Cooperation and social understanding in one- and two-year-olds. Child Development 77: 804–821.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, C., and M. Carriger. 1991. Collaborations among toddler peers: Individual contributions to social contexts. In Perspectives on socially shared cognition, ed. L. Resnick and J. Levine, 365–383. Washington: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, M., and P. Luetkenhaus. 1988. The development of volitional behavior in the toddler years. Child Development 59(3): 664–674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterfill, S. 2001. Two kinds of purposive action. European Journal of Philosophy 9(2): 141–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterfill, S. 2007. Joint action and communication in early childhood. Paper presented at British Psychological Society, University of Plymouth, Plymouth, England.

  • Carpendale, J.I.M., and C. Lewis. 2004. Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children’s social interaction within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 79–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M. 2009. Just How Joint Is Joint Action in Infancy? Topics in Cognitive Science 1(2): 380–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., K. Nagell, and M. Tomasello. 1998. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 63(4), serial no 255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T., and J. Bargh. 1999. The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76: 893–910.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. 2006. Social actions, social commitments. In Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition & interaction, ed. N. Enfield and S. Levinson. Oxford: Berg Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deák, G.O., R.A. Flom, and A.D. Pick. 2000. Effects of gesture and target on 12- and 18-month-olds’ joint visual attention to objects in front of or behind them. Developmental Psychology 36(4): 511–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dix, T., N. Cheng, and W. Day. 2009. Connecting with parents: Mothers’ depressive symptoms and responsive behaviors in the regulation of social contact by one- and young two-year-olds. Social Development 18(1): 24–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckerman, C.O., C.C. Davis, and S.M. Didow. 1989. Toddlers’ emerging ways of achieving social coordinations with a peer. Child Development 60(2): 440–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckerman, C.O., and K. Peterman. 2001. Peers and infant social/communicative development. In Blackwell handbook of infant development, ed. A. Fogel and G. Bremner, 326–350. Malden: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flom, R., and A.D. Pick. 2003. Verbal encouragement and joint attention in 18-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development 26(2): 121–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A. 2001. A relational perspective on development of self and emotion. In Identity & emotion, ed. B. Harke, 93–119. NY: Cambridge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, M. 2009. Shared intention and personal intentions. Philosophical Studies 144(1): 167–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grafenhain, M., T. Behne, M. Carpenter, and M. Tomasello. 2009. Young children’s understanding of joint commitments. Developmental Psychology 45(5): 1430–1443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamann, K., F. Warneken, and M. Tomasello. In press. Children's developing commitments to joint goals. Child Development.

  • Hay, D.F. 1979. Cooperative interactions and sharing between very young children and their parents. Developmental Psychology 15(6): 647–653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R., and G. Chidambi, et al. 2006. Foundations for self awareness: An exploration through autism. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 71 (Serial no. 284).

  • Holmberg, M. 1980. The development of social interchange patterns from 12 to 42 months. Child Development 51: 448–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunnius, S., H. Bekkering, and A. Cillessen. 2009. The association between intention understanding and peer cooperation in toddlers. European Journal of Developmental Science 3(4): 278–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, J. 1981. The second year: Emergence of self-awareness. Cambridge: MA, Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1992. Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knoblich, G., and N. Sebanz. 2008. Evolving intentions for social interaction: From entrainment to joint action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 363: 2021–2031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liszkowski, U., M. Carpenter, T. Striano, and M. Tomasello. 2006. 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development 7: 173–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcovitch, S., and P.D. Zelazo. 2009. A hierarchical competing systems model of the emergence and early development of executive function. Developmental Science 12(1): 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. 2007. Understanding self and others in the second year. In Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations, ed. C.A. Brownell and C.B. Kopp, 43–65. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C., and P.J. Dunham. 1995. Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., and J. Willoughby. 1996. Nonverbal communication, joint attention, and early socioemotional development. In Emotional development in atypical children, ed. M. Lewis, 65–87. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J. 1991. Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. 1952. The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. 2008. How infants know minds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheingold, H.L., D.F. Hay, and M.J. West. 1976. Sharing in the second year of life. Child Development 47: 1148–1158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, H.S., and S.P. Lollis. 1987. Communication within infant social games. Developmental Psychology 23(2): 241–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, D.R., and K. Durkin. 1987. Turn-taking in mother-infant interaction: An examination of vocalizations and gaze. Developmental Psychology 23(1): 54–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samson, D., and I. Apperly. 2010. There is more to mind reading than having theory of mind concepts: New directions in theory of mind research. Infant and Child Development 19: 443–454.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. 1990. Collective intentions and actions. In Intentions in communication, ed. P. Cohen, J. Morgan, and M. Pollack, 401–415. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., H. Bekkering, and G. Knoblich. 2006. Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Science 10(2): 70–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., and G. Knoblich. 2009. Prediction in joint action: What, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science 1: 353–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shintel, H., and B. Keysar. 2009. Less is more: A minimalist account of joint action in communication. Topics in Cognitive Science 1: 260–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smiley, P.A. 2001. Intention understanding and partner-sensitive behaviors in young children’s peer interactions. Social Development 10(3): 330–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinwender, J., F. Warneken, and M. Tomasello. 2010. Development of individual and collaborative problem-solving. Baltimore: Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D.N. 2002. The first relationship: Infant and mother. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tollefsen, D. 2005. Let’s pretend! Children and joint action. Philosophy of the Social Sciences 35(1): 75–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., M. Carpenter, J. Call, T. Behne, and H. Moll. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(5): 675–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. 1979. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication, ed. M. Bullowa, 321–380. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tuomela, R. 2005. We-intentions revisited. Philosophical Studies 125(3): 327–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.S. 1978. Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warneken, F., F. Chen, and M. Tomasello. 2006. Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Development 77(3): 640–663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P.D. 2004. The development of conscious control in childhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(1): 12–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Celia A. Brownell.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Brownell, C.A. Early Developments in Joint Action. Rev.Phil.Psych. 2, 193–211 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-011-0056-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-011-0056-1

Keywords

Navigation