Skip to main content
Log in

Mindfulness as Attention Training: Meta-Analyses on the Links Between Attention Performance and Mindfulness Interventions, Long-Term Meditation Practice, and Trait Mindfulness

  • REVIEW
  • Published:
Mindfulness Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives

To examine whether mindfulness training is associated with changes in objective attention performance.

Methods

Three meta-analyses were conducted: (a) a meta-analysis comparing the effects of mindfulness interventions with those of control treatments (109 effect sizes from 40 studies); (b) a meta-analysis comparing attention performance of long-term meditators with that of meditation-naïve participants (59 effect sizes from 18 studies); and (c) a meta-analysis comparing correlations between trait mindfulness and attention (197 effect sizes from 28 studies).

Results

Average effect sizes were significant (Hedges’ g = 0.29 for intervention studies, 0.32 for long-term meditation practice; r between trait mindfulness and attention = 0.12). All three analyses found significant effects on inhibition/executive control. Two out of the three analyses showed significant effects on updating and sustained attention. Shifting yielded significant effects only for interventions. Effects were larger for accuracy than for speed. Within intervention studies, focused attention led to significant effects, but inclusion of a yoga component decreased effects significantly. Number of sessions was positively related to attention performance. In long-term meditators, type of meditation practiced did not significantly moderate the effect size. Within trait mindfulness studies, only unidimensional measures and measures of acting with awareness yielded an average correlation that was significantly different from zero.

Conclusions

Mindfulness training as an intervention and a long-term practice is indeed associated with reliable changes in objective attention performance. The finding that this is also true for trait mindfulness suggests that mindfulness is a key mechanism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

(Studies included in the meta-analyses are indicated by an asterisk)

  • *Ainsworth, B., Eddershaw, R., Meron, D., Baldwin, D. S., & Garner, M. (2013). The effect of focused attention and open monitoring meditation on attention network function in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Research, 210, 1226-1231.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Allen, M., Dietz, M., Blair, K. S., van Beek, M., Rees, G., Vestergaard-Poulsen, P., Lutz, A., & Roepstorff, A. (2012). Cognitive-affective neural plasticity following active-controlled mindfulness intervention. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 15601-15610.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Anderson, N. D., Lau, M. A., Segal, Z. V., & Bishop, S. R. (2007). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and attentional control. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy: An International Journal of Theory & Practice, 14, 449-463.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Andreu, C. I., Moënne-Loccoz, C., López, V., Slagter, H. A., Franken, I. H., & Cosmelli, D. (2017). Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence of enhanced performance monitoring in meditators. Mindfulness, 8, 1603-1614.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Anicha, C. L., Ode, S., Moeller, S. K., & Robinson, M. D. (2012). Toward a cognitive view of trait mindfulness: distinct cognitive skills predict its observing and nonreactivity facets. Journal of Personality, 80, 255-285.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Assessment, 11, 191–206.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27–45.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Banks, J. B., Welhaf, M. S., & Srour, A. (2015). The protective effects of brief mindfulness meditation training. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 277-285.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Becerra, R., Dandrade, C., & Harms, C. (2017). Can specific attentional skills be modified with mindfulness training for novice practitioners?. Current Psychology, 36, 657-664.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Beck, A. R., Verticchio, H., Seeman, S., Milliken, E., & Schaab, H. (2017). A mindfulness practice for communication sciences and disorders undergraduate and speech-language pathology graduate students: effects on stress, self-compassion, and perfectionism. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 26, 893-907.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bergomi, C., Tschacher, W., & Kupper, Z. (2015). Meditation practice and self-reported mindfulness: a cross-sectional investigation of meditators and non-meditators using the comprehensive inventory of mindfulness experiences (CHIME). Mindfulness, 6, 1411–1421.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Bhayee, S., Tomaszewski, P., Lee, D. H., Moffat, G., Pino, L., Moreno, S., & Farb, N. A. (2016). Attentional and affective consequences of technology supported mindfulness training: a randomised, active control, efficacy trial. BMC psychology, 4, 60.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Black, D. S., Semple, R. J., Pokhrel, P., & Grenard, J. L. (2011). Component processes of executive function—mindfulness, self-control, and working memory—and their relationships with mental and behavioral health. Mindfulness, 2, 179-185.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bopp, K. L., & Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal memory span: a meta-analysis. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60, 223–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cardaciotto, L., Herbert, J. D., Forman, E. M., Moitra, E., & Farrow, V. (2008). The assessment of present-moment awareness and acceptance: the Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale. Assessment, 15, 204–223.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Cardeña, E., Sjöstedt, J. O., & Marcusson-Clavertz, D. (2015). Sustained attention and motivation in Zen meditators and non-meditators. Mindfulness, 6, 1082-1087.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 303-322.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Chan, D., & Woollacott, M. (2007). Effects of level of meditation experience on attentional focus: is the efficiency of executive or orientation networks improved?. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 13(6), 651-658.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Chang, J. H., Kuo, C. Y., Huang, C. L., & Lin, Y. C. (2018). The flexible effect of mindfulness on cognitive control. Mindfulness, 9, 792-800.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, M. W. L. (2014). Modeling dependent effect sizes with three-level meta-analyses: a structural equation modeling approach. Psychological Methods, 19, 211–229.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, M. W. L. (2015). metaSEM: an R package for meta-analysis using structural equation modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1521.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chiesa, A., & Malinowski, P. (2011). Mindfulness-based approaches: are they all the same? Journal of Clinical Psychology, 67, 404–424.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiesa, A., Calati, R., & Serretti, A. (2011). Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 449–464.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Course-Choi, J., Saville, H., & Derakshan, N. (2017). The effects of adaptive working memory training and mindfulness meditation training on processing efficiency and worry in high worriers. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 89, 1-13.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Di Francesco, S. A., Simione, L., López-Ramón, M. F., Belardinelli, M. O., Lupiáñez, J., & Raffone, A. (2017). Dispositional mindfulness facets predict the efficiency of attentional networks. Mindfulness, 8, 101-109.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Dubert, C. J., Schumacher, A. M., Locker, L., Gutierrez, A. P., & Barnes, V. A. (2016). Mindfulness and emotion regulation among nursing students: investigating the mediation effect of working memory capacity. Mindfulness, 7, 1061-1070.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot—based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics, 56, 455–463.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eberth, J., & Sedlmeier, P. (2012). The effects of mindfulness meditation: a meta-analysis. Mindfulness, 3, 174–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Erskine, J. A., Georgiou, G. J., Joshi, M., Deans, A., & Colegate, C. (2017). Ageing and thought suppression performance: its relationship with working memory capacity, habitual thought suppression and mindfulness. Consciousness and Cognition, 53, 211-221.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Esch, T., Winkler, J., Auwärter, V., Gnann, H., Huber, R., & Schmidt, S. (2017). Neurobiological aspects of mindfulness in pain autoregulation: unexpected results from a randomized-controlled trial and possible implications for meditation research. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 674.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Fabio, R. A., & Towey, G. E. (2018). Long-term meditation: the relationship between cognitive processes, thinking styles and mindfulness. Cognitive Processing, 19, 73-85.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Fan, Y., Tang, Y. Y., Tang, R., & Posner, M. I. (2015). Time course of conflict processing modulated by brief meditation training. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 911.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2007). Mindfulness and emotion regulation: the development and initial validation of the Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale-Revised (CAMS-R). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 29, 177–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Flook, L., Goldberg, S. B., Pinger, L., Bonus, K., & Davidson, R. J. (2013). Mindfulness for teachers: a pilot study to assess effects on stress, burnout, and teaching efficacy. Mind, Brain, and Education, 7, 182-195.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Fountain-Zaragoza, S., Londeree, A., Whitmoyer, P., & Prakash, R. S. (2016). Dispositional mindfulness and the wandering mind: implications for attentional control in older adults. Consciousness and Cognition, 44, 193-204.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, K. C., Nijeboer, S., Dixon, M. L., Floman, J. L., Ellamil, M., Rumak, S. P., Sedlmeier, P., & Christoff, K. (2014). Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 43, 48–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Galla, B. M., Hale, T. S., Shrestha, A., Loo, S. K., & Smalley, S. L. (2012). The disciplined mind: associations between the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills and attention control. Mindfulness, 3, 95-103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gamboz, N., Zamarian, S., & Cavallero, C. (2010). Age-related differences in the attention network test (ANT). Experimental Aging Research, 36, 287–305.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Giannandrea, A., Simione, L., Pescatori, B., Ferrell, K., Belardinelli, M. O., Hickman, S. D., & Raffone, A. (2019). Effects of the mindfulness-based stress reduction program on mind wandering and dispositional mindfulness facets. Mindfulness, 10, 185-195.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goyal, M., Singh, S., Sibinga, E. M., Gould, N. F., Rowland-Seymour, A., Sharma, R., Berger, Z., Sleicher, D., Maron, D., Shihab, H. M., Ranasinghe, P. D., Linn, S., Saha, S., Bass, E., & Haythornthwaite, J. A. (2014). Meditation programs for psychological stress and well-being: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Internal Medicine, 174, 357–368.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Greenberg, J., Reiner, K., & Meiran, N. (2013). “Off with the old”: mindfulness practice improves backward inhibition. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 618.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Greenberg, J., Romero, V. L., Elkin-Frankston, S., Bezdek, M. A., Schumacher, E. H., & Lazar, S. W. (2019). Reduced interference in working memory following mindfulness training is associated with increases in hippocampal volume. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 13, 366-376.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (2015). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic press.

  • *Heeren, A., Van Broeck, N., & Philippot, P. (2009). The effects of mindfulness on executive processes and autobiographical memory specificity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 403-409.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgins, H. S., & Adair, K. C. (2010). Attentional processes and meditation. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 872–878.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Isbel, B., & Mahar, D. (2015). Cognitive mechanisms of mindfulness: a test of current models. Consciousness and Cognition, 38, 50-59.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Jensen, C. G., Vangkilde, S., Frokjaer, V., & Hasselbalch, S. G. (2012). Mindfulness training affects attention—or is it attentional effort?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 106-123.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 109-119.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & Gelfand, L. (2010). Examining the protective effects of mindfulness training on working memory capacity and affective experience. Emotion, 10(1), 54-64.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Jha, A. P., Morrison, A. B., Dainer-Best, J., Parker, S., Rostrup, N., & Stanley, E. A. (2015). Minds “at attention”: mindfulness training curbs attentional lapses in military cohorts. PloS One, 10, e0116889.

  • *Jo, H. G., Malinowski, P., & Schmidt, S. (2017). Frontal theta dynamics during response conflict in long-term mindfulness meditators. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 11, 299.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Josefsson, T., & Broberg, A. (2011). Meditators and non-meditators on sustained and executive attentional performance. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 14, 291-309.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Josefsson, T., Lindwall, M., & Broberg, A. G. (2014). The effects of a short-term mindfulness based intervention on self-reported mindfulness, decentering, executive attention, psychological health, and coping style: examining unique mindfulness effects and mediators. Mindfulness, 5, 18-35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clinical psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 144–156.

  • Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 637–671.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karbach, J., & Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: a meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychological Science, 25, 2027–2037.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Kozasa, E. H., Sato, J. R., Lacerda, S. S., Barreiros, M. A., Radvany, J., Russell, T. A., Sanchez, L. G., Mello, L. E. A. M., & Amaro Jr, E. (2012). Meditation training increases brain efficiency in an attention task. Neuroimage, 59, 745-749.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lao, S. A., Kissane, D., & Meadows, G. (2016). Cognitive effects of MBSR/MBCT: a systematic review of neuropsychological outcomes. Consciousness and Cognition, 45, 109–123.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Lee, Y. C., & Chao, H. F. (2012). The role of active inhibitory control in psychological well-being and mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 618-621.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Lee, T. M., Leung, M. K., Hou, W. K., Tang, J. C., Yin, J., So, K. F., Lee, C. F. & Chan, C. C. H. (2012). Distinct neural activity associated with focused-attention meditation and loving-kindness meditation. PloS one, 7, e40054.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of psychological, educational, and behavioral treatment: confirmation from meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 48, 1181–1209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 12, 163–169.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Lykins, E. L., Baer, R. A., & Gottlob, L. R. (2012). Performance-based tests of attention and memory in long-term mindfulness meditators and demographically matched nonmeditators. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36, 103-114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, P. (2013). Neural mechanisms of attentional control in mindfulness meditation. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Malinowski, P., Moore, A. W., Mead, B. R., & Gruber, T. (2017). Mindful aging: the effects of regular brief mindfulness practice on electrophysiological markers of cognitive and affective processing in older adults. Mindfulness, 8, 78-94.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Mallya, S., & Fiocco, A. J. (2016). Effects of mindfulness training on cognition and well-being in healthy older adults. Mindfulness, 7, 453-465.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, C. J., Burgard, M., Mena, M., Abbasi, I., Bernhardt, N., Clemens, S., Curtis, E., Daggett, E., Hauch, J., Housh, K., Janz, A., Lindstrum, A., Luttropp, K., & Williamson, R. (2011). Short-term training in loving-kindness meditation produces a state, but not a trait, alteration of attention. Mindfulness, 2, 143–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Meland, A., Ishimatsu, K., Pensgaard, A. M., Wagstaff, A., Fonne, V., Garde, A. H., & Harris, A. (2015). Impact of mindfulness training on physiological measures of stress and objective measures of attention control in a military helicopter unit. The International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 25, 191-208.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49, 270–291.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Melloni, M., Sedeño, L., Couto, B., Reynoso, M., Gelormini, C., Favaloro, R., Canales-Johnson, A., Sigman, M., Manes, F. & Ibanez, A. (2013). Preliminary evidence about the effects of meditation on interoceptive sensitivity and social cognition. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 9, 47.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moeyaert, M., Ugille, M., Natasha Beretvas, S., Ferron, J., Bunuan, R., & Van den Noortgate, W. (2017). Methods for dealing with multiple outcomes in meta-analysis: a comparison between averaging effect sizes, robust variance estimation and multilevel meta-analysis. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 20, 559–572.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine, 6(7).

  • *Moore, A., & Malinowski, P. (2009). Meditation, mindfulness and cognitive flexibility. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 176-186.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Moore, A. W., Gruber, T., Derose, J., & Malinowski, P. (2012). Regular, brief mindfulness meditation practice improves electrophysiological markers of attentional control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 18.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, S. B. (2008). Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs. Organizational Research Methods, 11(2), 364–386.

  • *Morrison, A. B., Goolsarran, M., Rogers, S. L., & Jha, A. P. (2014). Taming a wandering attention: short-form mindfulness training in student cohorts. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 897.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Moynihan, J. A., Chapman, B. P., Klorman, R., Krasner, M. S., Duberstein, P. R., Brown, K. W., & Talbot, N. L. (2013). Mindfulness-based stress reduction for older adults: effects on executive function, frontal alpha asymmetry and immune function. Neuropsychobiology, 68, 34-43.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering. Psychological Science, 24, 776-781.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Noone, C., & Hogan, M. J. (2018). A randomised active-controlled trial to examine the effects of an online mindfulness intervention on executive control, critical thinking and key thinking dispositions in a university student sample. BMC Psychology, 6, 13.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Noone, C., Bunting, B., & Hogan, M. J. (2016). Does mindfulness enhance critical thinking? Evidence for the mediating effects of executive functioning in the relationship between mindfulness and critical thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 2043.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Ostafin, B. D., Kassman, K. T., & Wessel, I. (2013). Breaking the cycle of desire: Mindfulness and executive control weaken the relation between an implicit measure of alcohol valence and preoccupation with alcohol-related thoughts. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 27, 1153-1158.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peng, P., & Miller, A. C. (2016). Does attention training work? A selective meta-analysis to explore the effects of attention training and moderators. Learning and Individual Differences, 45, 77–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Prakash, R. S., Hussain, M. A., & Schirda, B. (2015). The role of emotion regulation and cognitive control in the association between mindfulness disposition and stress. Psychology and Aging, 30, 160-171

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Prätzlich, M., Kossowsky, J., Gaab, J., & Krummenacher, P. (2016). Impact of short-term meditation and expectation on executive brain functions. Behavioural Brain Research, 297, 268-276.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Quan, P., Wang, W., Chu, C., & Hou, L. (2018). Seven days of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy improves attention and coping style. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 46, 421-430.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Rahl, H. A., Lindsay, E. K., Pacilio, L. E., Brown, K. W., & Creswell, J. D. (2017). Brief mindfulness meditation training reduces mind wandering: the critical role of acceptance. Emotion, 17, 224-230.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Rodrigues, G. S., & Jaeger, A. (2018). Source memory and cognitive control in Gurdjieff meditators. Mindfulness, 9, 1736-1747.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Rodriguez Vega, B., Melero-Llorente, J., Bayon Perez, C., Cebolla, S., Mira, J., Valverde, C., & Fernández-Liria, A. (2014). Impact of mindfulness training on attentional control and anger regulation processes for psychotherapists in training. Psychotherapy Research, 24, 202-213.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Ruocco, A. C., & Wonders, E. (2013). Delineating the contributions of sustained attention and working memory to individual differences in mindfulness. Personality and Individual Differences, 54, 226-230.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Sahdra, B. K., MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Shaver, P. R., Rosenberg, E. L., Jacobs, T. L., Zanesco, A. P., King, B. G., Aichele, S. R., Bridwell, A. A., Mangun, G. R., Lavy, S., Wallace, B. A., & Saron, C. D. (2011). Enhanced response inhibition during intensive meditation training predicts improvements in self-reported adaptive socioemotional functioning. Emotion, 11, 299-312.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Santorelli, S. F., Kabat-Zinn, J., Blacker, M., Meleo-Meyer, F., & Koerbel, L. (2017). Mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) authorized curriculum guide. In Center for Mindfulness in Medicine, Health Care, and Society (CFM). School: University of Massachusetts Medical.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Schmertz, S. K., Anderson, P. L., & Robins, D. L. (2009). The relation between self-report mindfulness and performance on tasks of sustained attention. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 31, 60-66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F., & Bühner, M. (2015). Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educational Psychologist, 50, 138–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedlmeier, P., Eberth, J., Schwarz, M., Zimmermann, D., Haarig, F., Jaeger, S., & Kunze, S. (2012). The psychological effects of meditation: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1139–1171.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Semple, R. J. (2010). Does mindfulness meditation enhance attention? A randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness, 1, 121-130.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Short, M. M., Mazmanian, D., Oinonen, K., & Mushquash, C. J. (2016). Executive function and self-regulation mediate dispositional mindfulness and well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 97-103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–1074.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sipe, W. E., & Eisendrath, S. J. (2012). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy: theory and practice. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 57, 63–69.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Francis, A. D., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2007). Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biol, 5, e138.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Smilek, D., Carriere, J. S., & Cheyne, J. A. (2010). Failures of sustained attention in life, lab, and brain: ecological validity of the SART. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2564–2570.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Sørensen, L., Osnes, B., Visted, E., Svendsen, J. L., Adolfsdottir, S., Binder, P. E., & Schanche, E. (2018). Dispositional mindfulness and attentional control: the specific association between the mindfulness facets of non-judgment and describing with flexibility of early operating orienting in conflict detection. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2359.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Staub, B., Doignon-Camus, N., Bacon, E., & Bonnefond, A. (2014). Investigating sustained attention ability in the elderly by using two different approaches: inhibiting ongoing behavior versus responding on rare occasions. Acta Psychologica, 146, 51–57.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sterne, J. A., Savović, J., Page, M., Elbers, R., Blencowe, N., Boutron, I., Cates, C. J., Cheng, H. Y., Corbett, M. S., Eldridge, S. M., Emberson, J. R., Hernán, M. A., Hopewell, S., Hróbjartsson, A., Junquira, D. R., Jüni, P., Kirkham, J. J., Lasserson, Y., Li, T., et al. (2019). RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 366, l4898.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., Yu, Q., Sui, D., Rothbart, M. K., Fan, M., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 17152-17156.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Teng, S. C., & Lien, Y. W. (2016). What Confucius practiced is good for your mind: examining the effect of a contemplative practice in Confucian tradition on executive functions. Consciousness and Cognition, 42, 204-215.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Teper, R., & Inzlicht, M. (2013). Meditation, mindfulness and executive control: the importance of emotional acceptance and brain-based performance monitoring. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 85-92.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Travis, F., & Shear, J. (2010). Focused attention, open monitoring and automatic self-transcending: categories to organize meditations from Vedic, Buddhist and Chinese traditions. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 1110–1118.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Tsai, M. H., & Chou, W. L. (2016). Attentional orienting and executive control are affected by different types of meditation practice. Consciousness and Cognition, 46, 110-126.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *Valentine, E. R., & Sweet, P. L. (1999). Meditation and attention: a comparison of the effects of concentrative and mindfulness meditation on sustained attention. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 2, 59-70.

    Google Scholar 

  • *van den Hurk, P. A., Giommi, F., Gielen, S. C., Speckens, A. E., & Barendregt, H. P. (2010). Greater efficiency in attentional processing related to mindfulness meditation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1168-1180.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Leeuwen, S., Müller, N. G., & Melloni, L. (2009). Age effects on attentional blink performance in meditation. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 593–599.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • *van Vugt, M. K., & Jha, A. P. (2011). Investigating the impact of mindfulness meditation training on working memory: a mathematical modeling approach. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 344-353.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Vugt, M. K., & Slagter, H. A. (2014). Control over experience? Magnitude of the attentional blink depends on meditative state. Consciousness and Cognition, 23, 32–39.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verhaeghen, P. (2011). Aging and executive control: reports of a demise greatly exaggerated. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 174–180.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Verhaeghen, P. (2017). Presence: how mindfulness and meditation shape your brain, mind, and life. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Verhoeven, J. E., Vrijsen, J. N., van Oostrom, I., Speckens, A. E., & Rinck, M. (2014). Attention effects of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy in formerly depressed patients. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 5(4), 414-424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wasylyshyn, C., Verhaeghen, P., & Sliwinski, M. J. (2011). Aging and task switching: a meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 26, 15–20.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Waszczuk, M. A., Zavos, H. M., Antonova, E., Haworth, C. M., Plomin, R., & Eley, T. C. (2015). A multivariate twin study of trait mindfulness, depressive symptoms, and anxiety sensitivity. Depression and Anxiety, 32, 254–261.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • *Whitmoyer, P., Fountain-Zaragoza, S., Andridge, R., Bredemeier, K., Londeree, A., Kaye, L., & Prakash, R. S. (2020). Mindfulness training and attentional control in older adults: a randomized controlled trial. Mindfulness, 11, 203-218.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Wittmann, M., Peter, J., Gutina, O., Otten, S., Kohls, N., & Meissner, K. (2014). Individual differences in self-attributed mindfulness levels are related to the experience of time and cognitive self-control. Personality and Individual Differences, 64, 41-45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zanto, T. P., & Gazzaley, A. (2014). Attention and ageing. In A. C. Nobre & S. Kastner (Eds.), Oxford library of psychology. The Oxford handbook of attention (pp. 927–971). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • *Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P. (2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: evidence of brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 597–605.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paul Verhaeghen.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Research Involving Human Participants

This paper describes three meta-analyses; this is a re-analysis of already published data; no human participants were involved.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(PDF 226 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Verhaeghen, P. Mindfulness as Attention Training: Meta-Analyses on the Links Between Attention Performance and Mindfulness Interventions, Long-Term Meditation Practice, and Trait Mindfulness. Mindfulness 12, 564–581 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01532-1

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01532-1

Keywords

Navigation