Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Mindfulness 3/2013

01-09-2013 | ORIGINAL PAPER

Observing as an Essential Facet of Mindfulness: A Comparison of FFMQ Patterns in Meditating and Non-Meditating Individuals

Auteurs: Josefine L. Lilja, Lars-Gunnar Lundh, Torbjörn Josefsson, Fredrik Falkenström

Gepubliceerd in: Mindfulness | Uitgave 3/2013

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

One of the most comprehensive measures of mindfulness is the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ) with five factors—Observing, Describing, Acting with awareness, Non-judging, and Non-reactivity. Hierarchical confirmatory factor analyses, however, have suggested that only four of the FFMQ factors (i.e. all except Observing) were components of “an overall mindfulness construct”—which is puzzling because Observing represents a core aspect of all definitions of mindfulness. The purpose of the present study was to approach this problem by a person-oriented approach, focusing on patterns on the FFMQ scales, rather than linear associations between them. Data on the FFMQ were collected on 817 individuals. Cluster analysis according to the LICUR procedure was used to group these participants in 13 clusters, according to their profiles of scores on the five FFMQ scales. Of the participants, 325 were categorized as meditators and 317 as non-meditators. To test hypotheses about the relation between Observing and mindfulness (which we assumed should be higher among meditators), the meditators/non-meditators categorization was cross-tabulated with the FFMQ clusters. The results showed that all clusters in which meditators were over-represented had high scores on Observing, and all clusters in which meditators were under-represented had low scores on Observing—which supports the hypothesis that mindfulness is related to high levels of Observing. The relationship between Observing and Non-judging, however, was found to be more complex than expected. The results are discussed in terms of mindfulness seen as a multidimensional skill, which may develop differently in various subgroups of individuals.
Literatuur
go back to reference Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: The Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Assessment, 11(3), 191–206.PubMedCrossRef Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: The Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills. Assessment, 11(3), 191–206.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27–45.PubMedCrossRef Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13(1), 27–45.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, S., et al. (2008). Construct validity of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in meditating and nonmeditating samples. Assessment, 15(3), 329–342.PubMedCrossRef Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Lykins, E., Button, D., Krietemeyer, J., Sauer, S., et al. (2008). Construct validity of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in meditating and nonmeditating samples. Assessment, 15(3), 329–342.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bergman, L. R. (1998). A pattern-oriented approach to studying individual development: Snapshots and processes. In R. B. Cairns, L. R. Bergman, & J. Kagan (Eds.), Methods and models for studying the individual (pp. 82–31). Thousand Oaks. Bergman, L. R. (1998). A pattern-oriented approach to studying individual development: Snapshots and processes. In R. B. Cairns, L. R. Bergman, & J. Kagan (Eds.), Methods and models for studying the individual (pp. 82–31). Thousand Oaks.
go back to reference Bergman, L. R., & El-Khouri, B. M. (1987). EXACON: a fortran 77 program for the exact analysis of single cells in a contingency table. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 47(1), 155–161. doi:10.1177/0013164487471024.CrossRef Bergman, L. R., & El-Khouri, B. M. (1987). EXACON: a fortran 77 program for the exact analysis of single cells in a contingency table. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 47(1), 155–161. doi:10.​1177/​0013164487471024​.CrossRef
go back to reference Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. C., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230–241.CrossRef Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. C., Carmody, J., et al. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 11(3), 230–241.CrossRef
go back to reference Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.PubMedCrossRef Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Falkenström, F. (2009). Studying mindfulness in experienced meditators: a quasi-experimental approach. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(3), 305–310.CrossRef Falkenström, F. (2009). Studying mindfulness in experienced meditators: a quasi-experimental approach. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(3), 305–310.CrossRef
go back to reference Hansen, E., Lundh, L. G., Homman, A., & Wångby-Lundh, M. (2009). Measuring mindfulness. Pilot studies with the Swedish versions of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) and the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS). Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 38(1), 2–15.PubMedCrossRef Hansen, E., Lundh, L. G., Homman, A., & Wångby-Lundh, M. (2009). Measuring mindfulness. Pilot studies with the Swedish versions of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) and the Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS). Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 38(1), 2–15.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York: Hyperion. Kabat-Zinn, J. (1994). Wherever you go, there you are: mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York: Hyperion.
go back to reference Lilja, J. L., Frodi-Lundgren, A., Johansson Hanse, J., Josefsson, T., Lundh, L.-G., Sköld, C., et al. (2011). Five facets of mindfulness questionnaire—reliability and factor structure: a Swedish version. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 40(4), 291–303. doi:10.1080/16506073.2011.580367.PubMedCrossRef Lilja, J. L., Frodi-Lundgren, A., Johansson Hanse, J., Josefsson, T., Lundh, L.-G., Sköld, C., et al. (2011). Five facets of mindfulness questionnaire—reliability and factor structure: a Swedish version. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 40(4), 291–303. doi:10.​1080/​16506073.​2011.​580367.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 504–511.PubMedCrossRef Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 504–511.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Smalley, S. L., Loo, S. K., Hale, S. H., Shrestha, A., & McGough, J. (2009). Mindfulness and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(10), 1087–1098.PubMedCrossRef Smalley, S. L., Loo, S. K., Hale, S. H., Shrestha, A., & McGough, J. (2009). Mindfulness and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65(10), 1087–1098.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(2), 353–357.PubMedCrossRef Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(2), 353–357.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82(1), 1–8.PubMedCrossRef Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82(1), 1–8.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wishart, D. (1987). CLUSTAN. User manual. Cluster analysis software. Computing Laboratory, University of St. Andrews. Wishart, D. (1987). CLUSTAN. User manual. Cluster analysis software. Computing Laboratory, University of St. Andrews.
Metagegevens
Titel
Observing as an Essential Facet of Mindfulness: A Comparison of FFMQ Patterns in Meditating and Non-Meditating Individuals
Auteurs
Josefine L. Lilja
Lars-Gunnar Lundh
Torbjörn Josefsson
Fredrik Falkenström
Publicatiedatum
01-09-2013
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Mindfulness / Uitgave 3/2013
Print ISSN: 1868-8527
Elektronisch ISSN: 1868-8535
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-012-0111-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 3/2013

Mindfulness 3/2013 Naar de uitgave

ORTHOGONAL ROTATION IN CONSCIOUSNESS

Airscape