Skip to main content
Log in

The impact of global and local context information on the processing of deceptive actions in game sports

Higher head-fake frequencies and head-fake repetitions reduce the size of the head-fake effect in basketball

Einfluss der globalen und lokalen Kontextinformationen auf die Verarbeitung von Täuschungshandlungen in Spielsportarten

Höhere Blicktäuschungshäufigkeiten und Wiederholungen vermindern den Blicktäuschungseffekt im Basketball

  • Hauptbeitrag
  • Published:
German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In basketball, the defender’s reaction to a pass is slower and more error prone if the attacking player gazes into the opposite direction. This so-called head-fake effect might be modulated by context information, that is, the frequency and the sequence of head fakes occurring. Accordingly, the present study investigates the head-fake effect for different frequency proportions (20% vs. 50% vs. 80%). The results show that the head-fake effect decreases when the head fake occurs more often. Furthermore, a potential influence of previous fake experience (i.e., congruency-sequence effect) on the size of the head-fake effect was investigated. Importantly, previous fake experience did not cause the frequency-based modulation of the general size of the head-fake effect. These findings bare important implications for sports practice, especially when it comes to instructing players about the tactical use of deceptive actions.

Zusammenfassung

Im Basketball ist die Reaktion des Verteidigers auf einen Pass langsamer und stärker fehlerbehaftet, wenn der angreifende Spieler in die entgegengesetzte Richtung schaut. Dieser sogenannte Blicktäuschungseffekt („head-fake effect“) könnte durch Kontextinformationen moduliert werden, genauer durch die Häufigkeit und Sequenz von Blicktäuschungen. Daher wurde in der vorliegenden Studie der Blicktäuschungseffekt bei unterschiedlichen Häufigkeitsverhältnissen (20 % vs. 50 % vs. 80 %) untersucht. Im Ergebnis nahm der Effekt mit steigender Häufigkeit der Blicktäuschungen ab. Des Weiteren wurde der potenzielle Einfluss vorausgegangener Täuschungserfahrungen, das heißt der Kongruenz-Sequenz-Effekt, auf die Größe des Blicktäuschungseffekts untersucht. Im Wesentlichen verursachten frühere Täuschungserfahrungen nicht die häufigkeitsbasierte Modulation der allgemeinen Größe des Blicktäuschungseffekts. Diese Ergebnisse sollten bedeutende Auswirkungen auf die sportliche Praxis haben, insbesondere auf die Anleitung von Spielern bezüglich der taktischen Nutzung von Täuschungshandlungen.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The instruction is identical with the one used in previous studies on the head-fake effect in basketball.

    (e.g., Weigelt et al., 2017; Kunde et al., 2011). It focuses on gaze direction (and not on head orientation), as gaze direction has been shown to be a special attentional cue, which triggers reflexive shifts of attention in an observer (Langton, Watt, & Bruce, 2000). Of course, both the head orientation and the gaze direction might be responsible for the head-fake effect.

References

  • Abernethy, B., Gill, D. P., Parks, S. L., & Packer, S. T. (2001). Expertise and the perception of kinematic and situational probability information. Perception, 30(2), 233–252.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, J. A. (1971). A closed-loop theory of motor learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 3(2), 111–149.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Alhaj Ahmad Alaboud, M., Steggemann, Y., Klein-Soetebier, T., Kunde, W., & Weigelt, M. (2012). Täuschungshandlungen im Sport. Eine experimentelle Untersuchung zur Wirkung der Häufigkeitsverteilung auf die Blicktäuschung im Basketball [Deceptive actions in sports: An experimental investigation on the influence of the frequency distribution oft he head fake in basketball. Zeitschrift für Sportpsychologie, 19(3), 110–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alhaj Ahmad Alaboud, M., Güldenpenning, I., Steggemann-Weinrich, Y., Kunde, W., & Weigelt, M. (2016). Täuschungshandlungen im Sport: Der Blicktäuschungseffekt im Basketball unter quasirealistischen Bedingungen [Deceptive actions in sports: The head-fake effect in basketball under quasi-realistic conditions. Sportwissenschaft, 46(3), 223–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. T., Wright, M. J., Jackson, R. C., & Abernethy, B. (2013). Neural bases for anticipation skill in soccer: an FMRI study. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 35, 98–109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brault, S., Bideau, B., Kulpa, R., & Craig, C. M. (2012). Detecting deception in movement: the case of the side-step in rugby. PLoS One, 7(6), 1–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cañal-Bruland, R., & Mann, D. L. (2015). Time to broaden the scope of research on anticipatory behavior: a case for the role of probabilistic information. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1518.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cañal-Bruland, R., & Schmidt, M. (2009). Response bias in judging deceptive movements. Acta Psychologica, 130(3), 235–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cañal-Bruland, R., van der Kamp, J., & van Kesteren, J. (2010). An examination of motor and perceptual contributions to the recognition of deception from others’ actions. Human Movement Science, 29(1), 94–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egner, T. (2014). Creatures of habit (and control): a multi-level learning perspective on the modulation of congruency effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1247.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Egner, T., Ely, S., & Grinband, J. (2010). Going, going, gone: characterizing the time-course of congruency sequence effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 154.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Farrow, D., & Reid, M. (2012). The contribution of situational probability information to anticipatory skill. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 15(4), 368–373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(4), 480–506.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, R. (2002). Behavior of college baseball players in a virtual batting task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1131–1148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Güldenpenning, I., Kunde, W., & Weigelt, M. (2017). How to trick your opponent: a review article on deceptive actions in interactive sports. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 917. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00917.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, G., Dawson, B., Lay, B., & Young, W. (2012). Effects of a feint on reactive agility performance. Journal of Sports Sciences, 30(8), 787–795.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, R. C., Warren, S., & Abernethy, B. (2006). Anticipation skill and susceptibility to deceptive movements. Acta Psychologica, 123(3), 355–371.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunde, W., Skirde, S., & Weigelt, M. (2011). Trust my face: cognitive factors of head fakes in sports. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17(2), 110–127.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Langton, S. R., Watt, R. J., & Bruce, V. (2000). Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(2), 50–59.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Loffing, F., & Cañal-Bruland, R. (2017). Anticipation in sport. Current Opinion in Psychology, 16, 6–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loffing, F., & Hagemann, N. (2014). On-court position influences skilled tennis players’ anticipation of shot outcome. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 36(1), 14–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loffing, F., Stern, R., & Hagemann, N. (2015). Pattern-induced expectation bias in visual anticipation of action outcomes. Acta Psychologica, 161, 45–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1980). Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: theory and data. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 523–553.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, D. G., & Mitterer, J. O. (1982). Selective and divided attention in a Stroop task. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 684–700.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, D. L., Schaefers, T., & Cañal-Bruland, R. (2014). Action preferences and the anticipation of action outcomes. Acta Psychologica, 152, 1–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, R., Horswill, M. S., Kronvall-Parkinson, M., Poulter, D. R., & McKenna, F. P. (2009). The effect of disguise on novice and expert tennis players’ anticipation ability. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21(2), 178–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, J. R., & Besner, D. (2008). The Stroop effect: Why proportion congruent has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(3), 514.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stürmer, B., Leuthold, H., Soetens, E., Schröter, H., & Sommer, W. (2002). Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(6), 1345–1363.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verguts, T., Notebaert, W., Kunde, W., & Wühr, P. (2011). Post-conflict slowing: cognitive adaptation after conflict processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 76–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weigelt, M., Güldenpenning, I., Steggemann-Weinrich, Y., Alhaj Ahmad Alaboud, M., & Kunde, W. (2017). Control over the processing of the opponent’s gaze direction in basketball experts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,, 24(3), 828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wendt, M., & Luna-Rodriguez, A. (2009). Conflict-frequency affects flanker interference: role of stimulus-ensemble-specific practice and flanker-response contingencies. Experimental Psychology, 56(3), 206–217.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, M. J., & Jackson, R. C. (2014). Deceptive body movements reverse spatial cueing in soccer. PLoS One, 9(8), 1–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wühr, P., & Kunde, W. (2008). Pre-cueing spatial S‑R correspondence: Is there regulation of expected response conflict? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(4), 872–883.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This work was supported by a grant of the German Research Foundation (DFG) awarded to the first author (GU 1683/1-1) and the third author (KU 1964/13-1) of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Iris Güldenpenning.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

I. Güldenpenning, M. Alhaj Ahmad Alaboud, W. Kunde, and M. Weigelt declare that they have no competing interests.

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Caption Electronic Supplementary Material

Example of the stimulus material used in the study 1

Example of the stimulus material used in the study 2

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Güldenpenning, I., Alaboud, M.A.A., Kunde, W. et al. The impact of global and local context information on the processing of deceptive actions in game sports. Ger J Exerc Sport Res 48, 366–375 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12662-018-0493-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12662-018-0493-4

Keywords

Schlüsselwörter

Navigation