Skip to main content
Log in

Effect of Cue Characteristics on Event Based Prospective Memory: Evaluating Valence and Presentation Mode

  • Research in Progress
  • Published:
Psychological Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the effect of cue distinctiveness (presentation mode & valence) on event based prospective memory. It involved a 2 (positive, negative) x 2 (implicit, explicit) mixed design with repeated measure on the second factor. Cue (emotional & neutral words) learning preceded the ongoing task (vowel counting task). Participants (N = 40) made prospective memory response (press "p") to indicate cue occurrence during the ongoing task. Arial size 8 - gray on black at 120ms/word (implicit), - white on black at 250ms/word (explicit). Significant main effects of emotional valence and significant interaction between valence and modes of presentation was found. Post-hoc analysis revealed greater degree of accuracy for positive cues only during implicit presentations. The study concludes that cue valence functions as a measure of cue distinctiveness in event based prospective memory. Presentation modes particularly the implicit mode differentially benefits only the positive valence cue.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Altgassen, M., Kliegel, M., & Martin, M. (2009). Event-based prospective memory in depression: the impact of cue focality. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1041–1055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Altgassen, M., Philips, L., Henry, J., Rendell, P., & Kliegel, M. (2010). Emotional target cues eliminate age differences in prospective memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1057–1064.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, R. (1998). Physiological measures of emotion. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 15, 388–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bargh, J., & Morsella, E. (2008). The unconscious mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(1), 73–79.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bernat, E., Bunce, S., & Shevrin, H. (2001). Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42, 11–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C., & Damasio, A. (1995). Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269, 1115–1118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M., & Lang, P. (1999). Affective norms for english words (ANEW): Instruction manual and affective ratings, Technical Report C-1, The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida.

  • Bunce, S., Bernat, E., Wong, P., & Shevrin, H. (1999). Further evidence for unconscious learning: preliminary support for the conditioning of facial EMG to subliminal stimuli. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 33, 341–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark-Foos, A., Brewer, G., Marsh, R., Meeks, J., & Cook, G. (2009). The valence of event-based prospective memory cues or the context in which they occur affect their detection. American Journal of Psychology, 122, 89–97.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes' error. New York:Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dobbs, A., & Reeves, M. (1996). Prospective memory: More than memory. In M. Brandimonte, G. Einstien, & M. McDaneil (Eds.), Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications (pp. 199–225). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., & Schlaghecken, F. (2003). Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming. Biological Psychology, 64(1–2), 7–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709–724.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Esteves, F., Parra, C., Dimberg, U., & Ohman, A. (1994). Nonconscious associative learning: pavolvian conditioning of skin conductance responses to masked fear-relevant facial stimuli. Psychophysiology, 31, 375–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, C., & Hansen, R. (1988). Finding the face in the crowd: an anger superiority effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 917–924.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hashimoto, T., Umeda, S., & Kojima, S. (2011). Neural substrates of implicit cuing effect on prospective memory. NeuroImage, 54, 645–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henke, K. (2010). A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(7), 523–532.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. (1993). Four systems for emotion activation: cognitive and non-cognitive processes. Psychological Review, 100, 68–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom, J. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237, 1445–1452.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kunst-Wilson, W., & Zajonc, R. (1980). Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, 557–558.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P., Greenwald, M., Bradley, M., & Hamm, A. (1993). Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral manifestations. Psychophysiology, 30, 261273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 352–367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. (1989). Cognitive–emotional interactions in the brain. Cognition and Emotion, 3(4), 267–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDaniel, M., & Einstein, G. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: a multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 127–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merikle, P., & Smith, S. (2005). Memory for information perceived without awareness. In N. Ohta, C. M. Macleod, & B. Uttl (Eds.), Dynamic cognitive processes (pp. 79–99). Tokyo: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Meneguzzo, P., Tsakiris, M., Schioth, H., Stein, J., & Brooks, S. (2014). Subliminal versus supraliminal neural responses in anterior cingulate cortex, fusiform gyrus and insula: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. BMC Psychology, 2, 52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. (1996). How we think about cognition, emotion, and biology in psychopathology. Psychophysiology, 33, 615–628.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, J., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. (1998). Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature, 393, 467–470.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B., Williams, R.., & Mathews, A. (1993). Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 304–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, N., & Isaacowitz, D. (2008). Preferences for emotional information in older and younger adults: a meta-analysis of memory and attention tasks. Psychology and Aging, 23, 263–286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Muscarella, C., Brintazzoli, G., Gordts, S., Soetens, E., & Van den Bussche, E. (2013). Short and long term effects of conscious, minimally conscious and unconscious brand logos. PloS One, 8, e57738.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ochsner, K., Silvers, J., & Buhle, J. (2012). Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: a systematic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251, E1–E24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ohman, A., Lundqvist, D., & Esteves, F. (2001). The face in the crowd revisited: threat advantage with schematic stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 381–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohman, A., Esteves, F., & Soares, J. (1995). Preparedness and pre-attentive associative learning: electrodermal conditioning to masked stimuli. Journal of Psychophysiology, 9(2), 99–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratto, F., & John, O. (1991). Automatic vigilance: the attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 380–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, C., & Jastrow, J. (1884). On small differences in sensation. Memoirs of the National Academy of Science, 3, 75–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rauch, S., Whalen, P., Shin, L., McInerney, S., Macklin, M., Lasko, N., Orr, S., & Pitman, R. (2000). Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 769–776.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rendell, P., Phillips, L., Henry, J., Brumby-Rendell, T., Piedad Garcia, X., Altgassen, M., & Liegel, M. (2011). Prospective memory, emotional valence & aging. Cognition and Emotion, 25(5), 916–925.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rude, S., Hertel, P., Jarrold, W., Covich, J., & Hedlund, S. (1999). Depression related impairments in prospective memory. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 267–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. (1978). Evidence of convergent validity on the dimensions of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1152–1168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shalgi, S., & Deouell, L. (2013). Is there any electrophysiological evidence for subliminal error processing? Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 150.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shevrin, H., & Fritzler, D. (1968). Visual evoked response correlates of unconscious mental processes. Science, 161, 295–298.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shevrin, H., & Dickman, S. (1980). The psychological unconscious, a necessary assumption for all psychological theory? American. Psychologist, 35, 421–435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shevrin, H., Bond, J., Brakel, L., Hertel, R., & Williams, W. (1996). Conscious and unconscious processes: Psychodynamic, cognitive, and neurophysiological convergences. New York:Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turk-Browne, N., Yi, D., Leber, A., & Chun, M. (2007). Visual quality determines the direction of neural repetition effects. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 425–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tsushima, Y., Sasaki, Y., & Watanabe, T. (2006). Greater disruption due to failure of inhibitory control on an ambiguous distractor. Science, 314, 1786–1788.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Anna Clark, L., & Tellegen, A. (1998). Development and validation of brief measures of postive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

  • Whalen, P., Rauch, S., Etcoff, N., McInerney, S., Lee, M., & Jenike, M. (1998). Masked presentation of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 411–418.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, P., Shevrin, H., & Williams, W. (1994). Conscious and nonconscious processes: an ERP index of an anticipatory response in a conditioning paradigm using visually masked stimuli. Psychopsysiology, 31, 87–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, P., Bernat, E., Bunce, S., & Shevrin, H. (1997). Brain indices of nonconscious associative learning. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 519–544.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, J., Xu, X., Du, X., Shi, C., & Fang, F. (2011). Effects of unconscious processing on implicit memory for fearful faces. PloS One, 6(2), e14641.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to N. Kashyap.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Singh, T., Kashyap, N. Effect of Cue Characteristics on Event Based Prospective Memory: Evaluating Valence and Presentation Mode. Psychol Stud 61, 13–20 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-015-0331-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12646-015-0331-9

Keywords

Navigation