Skip to main content
Log in

The Acuity for Numerosity (but Not Continuous Magnitude) Discrimination Correlates with Quantitative Problem Solving but Not Routinized Arithmetic

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Approximate number sense (ANS) acuity refers to the ability to non-symbolically recognize, estimate and operate upon large numerosities. ANS acuity has been reported to be correlated with math achievement in children and adolescents. However, reports of this relationship in adults have been inconsistent. The present study aimed to resolve the inconsistency in the relationship between adults’ ANS acuity and math achievement by contrasting between different kinds of mathematical problem solving. We hypothesized that the correlation between ANS acuity mathematical performance would be stronger when deep quantitative processing is required during problem solving. In Experiment 1, ANS acuity was correlated with Mathematical Reasoning but not Directed Calculation performance. In Experiment 2, ANS acuity was correlated with Two-digit Subtraction (but not Addition) performance only when Regrouping (i.e., borrowing) was required. The results from two experiments demonstrated that ANS acuity was correlated with mathematical performance only when problem solving involved effortful, quantitative processing that goes beyond automatized, routinized arithmetic. In addition, ANS acuity was distinguishable from Area acuity regarding its unique relationship with math achievement, which was unconfounded by the influence of demographic variables and fluid intelligence. Overall, the present results help resolve the inconsistency in previous reports of the correlation between ANS acuity and math achievement in adults.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Agrillo, C., Piffer, L., & Adriano, A. (2013). Individual differences in non-symbolic numerical abilities predict mathematical achievements but contradict ATOM. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 9(1), 26.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Arthur, W., Tubre, T. C., Paul, D. S., & Sanchez-Ku, M. L. (1999). College-sample psychometric and normative data on a short form of the Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices Test. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 17(4), 354–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ashkenazi, S., Golan, N., & Silverman, S. (2014). Domain-specific and domain-general effects on strategy selection in complex arithmetic: evidences from ADHD and normally developed college students. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 3(3), 93–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berch, D. B., & Mazzocco, M. M. (2007). Why is math so hard for some children? The nature and origins of mathematical learning difficulties and disabilities. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Pub. Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonny, J. W., & Lourenco, S. F. (2013). The approximate number system and its relation to early math achievement: evidence from the preschool years. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(3), 375–388.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brannon, E. M. (2006). The representation of numerical magnitude. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(2), 222–229.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B. (2005). Developmental dyscalculia. In J. I. D. Campbell (Ed.), Handbook of mathematical cognition (pp. 455–467). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

  • Cantlon, J. F., Platt, M. L., & Brannon, E. M. (2009). Beyond the number domain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 83.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Castronovo, J., & Göbel, S. M. (2012). Impact of high mathematics education on the number sense. PLoS One, 7(4), e33832.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, S., Metcalfe, A. W., Young, C. B., Ryali, S., Geary, D. C., & Menon, V. (2012). Hippocampal–prefrontal engagement and dynamic causal interactions in the maturation of children’s fact retrieval. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(9), 1849–1866.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, I. (2009). Korean teachers’ perceptions of student success in mathematics: concept versus procedure. The Montana Mathematics Enthusiast, 6(1–2), 239–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L., & Ghesquière, P. (2009). The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(4), 469–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1996). The organization of brain activations in number comparison: event-related potentials and the additive-factors methods. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 47–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2007). Symbols and quantities in parietal cortex: elements of a mathematical theory of number representation and manipulation. Attention & performance XXII. Sensori-motor foundations of higher cognition, ed. P. Haggard & Y. Rossetti, 527–74.

  • Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Stanescu, R., Pinel, P., & Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science, 284, 970–974.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., Molko, N., Cohen, L., & Wilson, A. J. (2004). Arithmetic and the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(2), 218–224.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeWind, N. K., & Brannon, E. M. (2012). Malleability of the approximate number system: effects of feedback and training. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 68.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Duffau, H., Denvil, D., Lopes, M., Gasparini, F., Cohen, L., Capelle, L., & Van Effenterre, R. (2002). Intraoperative mapping of the cortical areas involved in multiplication and subtraction: an electrostimulation study in a patient with a left parietal glioma. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 73(6), 733–738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Educational Testing Service (2010). The official guide to the GRE revised general test (2 ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

  • Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 307–314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gabrieli, J. D. (2009). Dyslexia: a new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience. Science, 325(5938), 280–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilmore, C. K., McCarthy, S. E., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Non-symbolic arithmetic abilities and achievement in the first year of formal schooling in mathematics. Cognition, 115(3), 394.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Grabner, R. H., Ansari, D., Reishofer, G., Stern, E., Ebner, F., & Neuper, C. (2007). Individual differences in mathematical competence predict parietal brain activation during mental calculation. NeuroImage, 38(2), 346–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grabner, R. H., Ansari, D., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., & Neuper, C. (2009). To retrieve or to calculate? Left angular gyrus mediates the retrieval of arithmetic facts during problem solving. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 604–608.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J., Hudson, C., & Price, D. (2009). The long term costs of numeracy difficulties. London: Every Child a Chance Trust and KPMG.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guillaume, M., Nys, J., & Mussolin, C. (2013). Differences in the acuity of the Approximate Number System in adults: The effect of mathematical ability. Acta Psychologica, 144(3), 506–512.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halberda, J., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Developmental change in the acuity of the “Number Sense”: the Approximate Number System in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1457.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halberda, J., Mazzocco, M., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Individual differences in non-verbal ANS acuity correlate with maths achievement. Nature, 455(7213), 665–668.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanushek, E. A., & Woessmann, L. (2010). The high cost of low educational performance: The long-run economic impact of improving PISA outcomes. Paris: OECD Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, D. C., Khanum, S., & Spelke, E. S. (2014). Brief non-symbolic, approximate number practice enhances subsequent exact symbolic arithmetic in children. Cognition, 131(1), 92–107.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Inglis, M., Attridge, N., Batchelor, S., & Gilmore, C. (2011). Non-verbal ANS acuity correlates with symbolic mathematics achievement: but only in children. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(6), 1222–1229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, V., Sann, C., Spelke, E. S., & Streri, A. (2009). Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(25), 10382–10385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiefer, M., & Dehaene, S. (1997). The time course of parietal activation in single-digit multiplication: evidence from event-related potentials. Mathematical Cognition, 3, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leung, F. K. (2001). In search of an East Asian identity in mathematics education. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 47(1), 35–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leung, F., & Park, K. (2002). Competent students, competent teachers? International Journal of Educational Research, 37(2), 113–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011). Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. Developmental Science, 14(6), 1292–1300.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Libertus, M. E., Odic, D., & Halberda, J. (2012). Intuitive sense of number correlates with math scores on college-entrance examination. Acta Psychologica, 141(3), 373–379.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lindskog, M., Winman, A., Juslin, P., & Poom, L. (2013). Measuring acuity of the approximate number system reliably and validly: the evaluation of an adaptive test procedure. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 510.

  • Lipton, J. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2003). Origins of number sense large-number discrimination in human infants. Psychological Science, 14(5), 396–401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lourenco, S. F., Bonny, J. W., Fernandez, E. P., & Rao, S. (2012). Nonsymbolic number and cumulative area representations contribute shared and unique variance to symbolic math competence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(46), 18737–18742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, I. M., & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Numerical ordering ability mediates the relation between number-sense and arithmetic competence. Cognition, 121(2), 256–261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzocco, M., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011a). Impaired acuity of the approximate number system underlies mathematical learning disability (dyscalculia). Child Development, 82(4), 1224–1237.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzocco, M., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2011b). Preschoolers’ precision of the approximate number system predicts later school mathematics performance. PLoS One, 6(9), e23749.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Obersteiner, A., Reiss, K., & Ufer, S. (2013). How training on exact or approximate mental representations of number can enhance first-grade students’ basic number processing and arithmetic skills. Learning and Instruction, 23, 125–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Odic, D., Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., & Halberda, J. (2013). Developmental change in the acuity of approximate number and area representations. Developmental Psychology, 49(6), 1103.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Park, J., & Brannon, E. M. (2013). Training the approximate number system improves math proficiency. Psychological Science, 24(10), 2013–2019.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Park, J., & Brannon, E. M. (2014). Improving arithmetic performance with number sense training: an investigation of underlying mechanism. Cognition, 133(1), 188–200.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Park, K., Lee, H., Im, H. (2008). Test equating of paper-based and computer-based versions of the Korean Job Aptitude Scale for Adults. Seoul: Korea Employment Information Service.

  • Parsons, S., & Bynner, J. (2005). Does numeracy matter more? London: National Research and Development Centre for adult literacy and numeracy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science, 306(5695), 499–503.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pinel, P., Le Clec’H, G., van de Moortele, P. F., Naccache, L., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (1999). Event-related fMRI analysis of the cerebral circuit for number comparison. NeuroReport, 10(7), 1473–1479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Price, G. R., Palmer, D., Battista, C., & Ansari, D. (2012). Nonsymbolic numerical magnitude comparison: reliability and validity of different task variants and outcome measures, and their relationship to arithmetic achievement in adults. Acta Psychologica, 140(1), 50–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Harskamp, N. J., & Cipolotti, L. (2001). Selective impairments for addition, subtraction and multiplication. Implications for the organisation of arithmetical facts. Cortex, 37(3), 363–388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vilette, B., Mawart, C., & Rusinek, S. (2010). L’outil «estimateur», la ligne numérique mentale et les habiletés arithmétiques. Pratiques Psychologiques, 16(2), 203–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483–488.

  • Zamarian, L., Ischebeck, A., & Delazer, M. (2009). Neuroscience of learning arithmetic—evidence from brain imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(6), 909–925.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from the National Research Foundation of Korea (2012R1A1A1011872, 2014R1A1A3051034) funded by the Korean government.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Soohyun Cho.

Appendix

Appendix

Measuring the Acuity for Numerosity Discrimination

Individual differences in numerosity and length acuity was calculated the Weber fraction coefficient (w) which based on a psychophysical model for numerosity comparison (Halberda et al. 2008; Pica et al. 2004). The formula of the psychophysics model is as follows:

$$ \mathrm{Proportion}\ \mathrm{Correct} = 1\kern0.5em -\kern0.5em \frac{1}{2}erf\kern0.5em c\left(\frac{{\mathrm{n}}_1-{\mathrm{n}}_2}{w\sqrt{2\left({{\mathrm{n}}_1}^2+{{\mathrm{n}}_2}^2\right)}}\right) $$

The ”erfc” is a complementary Gaussian error function given by:

$$ \mathrm{erfc}\left(\mathrm{x}\right)\kern0.5em =\kern0.5em 1\kern0.5em -\kern0.5em \frac{2}{\sqrt{\uppi}}{\displaystyle {\int}_0^{\mathrm{x}}{e}^{-{t}^2}dt} $$

This model fits the proportion correct when comparing between a pair of quantities or lines (n1 and n2). The resulting value of w for each individual served as an estimator of the ratio of n1 and n2 that one can readily differentiate. Lower w indicates a higher sensitivity of discrimination.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Jang, S., Cho, S. The Acuity for Numerosity (but Not Continuous Magnitude) Discrimination Correlates with Quantitative Problem Solving but Not Routinized Arithmetic. Curr Psychol 35, 44–56 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-015-9354-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-015-9354-6

Keywords

Navigation