Skip to main content
Log in

Corporeal Reflexivity and Autism

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Ethnographic video recordings of high functioning children with autism or Aspergers Syndrome in everyday social encounters evidence their first person perspectives. High quality visual and audio data allow detailed analysis of children’s bodies and talk as loci of reflexivity. Corporeal reflexivity involves displays of awareness of one’s body as an experiencing subject and a physical object accessible to the gaze of others. Gaze, demeanor, actions, and sotto voce commentaries on unfolding situations indicate a range of moment-by-moment reflexive responses to social situations. Autism is associated with neurologically based motor problems (e.g. delayed action-goal coordination, clumsiness) and highly repetitive movements to self-soothe. These behaviors can provoke derision among classmates at school. Focusing on a 9-year-old girl’s encounters with peers on the playground, this study documents precisely how autistic children can become enmeshed as unwitting objects of stigma and how they reflect upon their social rejection as it transpires. Children with autism spectrum disorders in laboratory settings manifest diminished understandings of social emotions such as embarrassment, as part of a more general impairment in social perspective-taking. Video ethnography, however, takes us further, into discovering autistic children’s subjective sense of vulnerability to the gaze of classmates.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Figs. 4–5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Figs. 8–9
Figs. 10–12
Figs. 13–15
Fig. 16

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. To confirm diagnosis, researchers administered the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI; Le Couteur et al. 1989) and the Autism Behavior Checklist (Krug et al. 1978). The children‶s abilities were assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale (WISC-III, Wechsler 1992), and a series of theory of mind tasks (Baron-Cohen 1989; Baron-Cohen et al. 1985; Happé 1994; Leslie and Frith 1988).

References

  • Agamben, G. (1993). The coming community (M. Hardy, Trans.). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Agamben, G. (1998). Homo sacer: Sovereign power and bare life. Palo Alto: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J.L. (1975). In J. O. Urmson, & M. Sbisà (Ed.), How to do things with words (Second Edition). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: a case of specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30(2), 285–297.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Lesie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J., Sommons, A., & Williams, S. C. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11(6), 1891–1898.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). The Essential Difference: The Truth about the Male and Female Brain. New York: Basic Books

  • Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. New York: Ballantine Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowler, D. M. (1992). “Theory of mind” in Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(5), 877–893.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: an investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33, 781–789.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (2009). The relevance of Hussert’s theory to language socialization. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 2, 205–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., Frith C.D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalising. Philosophical Transactions Royal Society of London, Series B., 358 (Special issue on Mechanisms of social interaction.), 459–473.

  • Foucault, M. (1988). Technologies of the self: A seminar with Michel Foucault. Amherst: The University of Massachusetts Press.

  • Garfinkel, H. (2006). Seeing sociologically: The routine grounds of social action. Boulder: Paradigm Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: notes on the management of spoiled identity. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, S., Wang, C., Salgado, M. W., Greene, P. E., Kim, M., & Rapin, I. (2008). Motor stereotypes in children with autism and other developmental disorders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 51(1), 30–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1994). Minds, bodies and persons: Young children’s understanding of the self and others as reflected in imitation and “theory of mind” research. In S. T. Parker & R. Mitchell (Eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gumperz, J. J. (1982). Discourse strategies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1994). Autism: an introduction to psychological theory. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1977). Phenomenological psychology: Lectures, summer semester, 1925. The Hague: Nijhoff.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Krug, D. A., Arick, J., & Almond, P. (1978). Autism behavior checklist. Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Couteur, A., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Rios, P., Robertson, S., Holdgrafer, M., & McLennan, J. (1989). Autism diagnostic interview: a standardized investigator-based instrument. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(3), 383–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A, & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children’s understanding of seeing, knowing and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6(4).

  • Link, B. C., & Phelan, J. (2001). Conceptualizing stigma. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 363–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., & Solomon, O. (2010). Autistic sociality. Ethos, 38(1), 69–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., Kremer-Sadlik, T., & Solomon, O. (2004). Autism and the social world: an anthropological perspective. Discourse Studies, 6(2), 147–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, O. (2011). Body in autism: A view from social interaction. In P. McPherron & V. Ramanathan (Eds.), Language, body, and health. Boston: Walter de Gruyton, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, O. (2012). Doing, being, becoming: the sociality of children with autism in activities with therapy dogs and other people. Cambridge Anthropology, 30(1), 109–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sterponi, L., & Shankey, J. (2013). Rethinking echolalia: repetition as interactional resource in the communication of a child with autism. Journal of Child Language, 41(02), 275–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M., & Carlson, S. (1997). The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind. Child Development, 68, 436–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.). Sidcup: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L. (1997). The history of ideas on autism. Autism, 1(1), 13–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (2008). Subjectivity and selfhood. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This analysis has benefitted from years of dialogue and collaboration with Olga Solomon, whose continuing research on the lived worlds of children with autism spectrum disorders is source of inspiration. I am also grateful to Rachel Flamenbaum for her assistance in presenting the video and audio data for this study. This research is part of a larger study ‘Socializing Autistic Children into the Rules of School and Family Life,’ funded by the Spencer Foundation for Educational and Related Research (E. Ochs, Principal Investigator 2000–2003). The data were collected during a Spencer-funded project ‘Autistic Children’s Narrative Interactions at School and Home,’ (E. Ochs and L. Capps, Co-Principal Investigators, 1997–2000).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elinor Ochs.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ochs, E. Corporeal Reflexivity and Autism. Integr. psych. behav. 49, 275–287 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-015-9306-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-015-9306-6

Keywords

Navigation