Skip to main content
Log in

The Rhythmic, Sonorous and Melodic Components of Adult-Child-Object Interactions Between 2 and 6 Months Old

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Adults mediate the relationship between material reality and children, according to functional units of cultural relevance. This paper explores early development of semiotic systems in infants, analyzing rhythmic, sonorous and melodic components, which enable adult-child interaction with and about objects. The triads (with sonorous and non-sonorous objects) was studied longitudinally at age 2, 4 and 6 months. We propose that rhythmic, sonorous and melodic components conformed one of the basic semiotic systems upon the adult's action relies (through gestures and uses of objects) in order to segment and organize objects in the world. Likewise, children actively respond to these presentations and seek sounds for themselves when they are able to interact with the object more autonomously.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Graph 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The same object was used in a case study of self-regulation function in private gestures on a 18 months old girl with Down Syndrome (Rodríguez and Palacios 2007).

  2. Glissandos are cascades of sounds produced by the adult in an ascending or descending manner.

  3. Ostensive gestures, as happens between adults, were eliminated from this classification because they are produced in a static manner and, therefore, they do not involve either rhythm, nor sound.

  4. Only the sequences of action with the sonorous rings were noted because the adult preferably used them.

References

  • Andrén, M. (2010). Children’s gestures from 18 to 30 months. Travaux de l’Institut de Linguistique de Lund 50. Lund: Lund University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basilio, M., & Rodríguez, C. (2011). Usos, gestos y vocalizaciones privadas. De la interacción social a la autorregulación [Uses, gestures and private vocalizations: from social interaction to self-regulation]. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 34(2), 181–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21(3), 205–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronckart, J. P. (2012). Contributions of piagetian constructivism to social interactionism. In E. Martí & C. Rodríguez (Eds.), After Piaget (pp. 43–58). New Jersey: Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bühler, K. (2009). Théorie du langage [Theory of Language]. Marsella: Agone.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cárdenas, K., Rodríguez, C., & Palacios, P. (2014). First symbols in a girl with down syndrome: a longitudinal study from 12 to 18 months-olds. Infant, Behavior and Development, 37(3), 416–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavalcante, S., & Rodríguez, C. (2015). La comprensión del dado como objeto con funciones numéricas. Un estudio longitudinal con dos niños desde los 24 a los 36 meses en interacción con un adulto. Monográfico: Instrumentos semióticos en el conocimiento matemático temprano. [The understanding of dice as an object that has numerical functions. A longitudinal study using two children from the ages of 24 to 36 months interacting with an adult. Special Issue: Semiotic tools in early mathematical knowledge] Estudios de Psicología, 36(1), 1-23. doi: 10.1080/02109395.2014.1000028

  • Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychological Assessment, 6(4), 284–290.

  • Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language, 25(2), 141–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Del Olmo, M. J. (2009). Musicoterapia con bebés de 0 a 6 meses en cuidados intensivos pediátricos [Music therapy with babies from 0- to 6-month-old in the PICU] (Tesis Doctoral inédita) [Unedited Doctoral Thesis]. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, España.

  • Del Olmo, M. J., Rodríguez, C., & Ruza, F. (2010). Music therapy in the PICU: 0- to 6-month-old babies. Music and Medicine, 2(3), 158–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Díaz, M. (2004). La educación musical en la etapa 0–6 años. [Musical education in the stage from 0 to 6 years] Revista Electrónica de LEEME (Lista Europea de Música en la Educación), 14, 1–10. Obtained from http://musica.rediris.es/leeme/revista/diazinf.pdf

  • Dimitrova, N. (2012). Développement de la communication intentionelle gestuelle à partir des usages culturels des objets dans l’interaction triadique enfant-objet-adulte [Development of intentional gestural communication from cultural uses of objects in the triadic interaction of child-adult-object] (Unedited PhD dissertation). Université de Lausanne, Switzerland.

  • Español, S. (2004). Cómo hacer cosas sin palabras: gesto y ficción en la infancia temprana [How to do things without words: Gestures and fiction in early infancy]. Madrid: Antonio Machado.

  • EUDICO linguistic annotator - Elan (Version 4.1.1) [Software computation] (2011). Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.

  • Freelon, D. G. (2010). ReCal: intercoder reliability calculation as a web service. International Journal of Internet Science, 5(1), 20–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Futó, J., Téglás, E., Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2010). Communicative function demonstration induces kind-based artifact representation in preverbal infants. Cognition, 117, 1–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hargreaves, D. J. (2002). Música y desarrollo psicológic [Music and psychological development]. Barcelona: Graó.

  • Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., Jasnow, M. D., Rochat, P., & Stern, D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: coordinated timing in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 66(2), 1–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leavens, D., Hopkins, W., & Bard, K. (2008). The heterochronic origins of explicit reference. In J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 187–214). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Liszkowski, U., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Pointing out new news, old news and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science, 10(2), F1–F7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2006). 12- and 18-months-olds point to provide information for others. Journal Child Language, 7(2), 173–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malloch, S., & Trevarthen, C. (Eds.). (2009). Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship. Nueva York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moro, C., & Rodríguez, C. (2005). L’objet et la construction de son usage chez le bébé. Une approche sémiotique du développement préverbal [The object and construction of its use in infants. A semiotic approach of preverbal development]. Bern-New York: Peter Lang.

  • Palacios, P., & Rodríguez, C. (2015). The development of symbolic uses of objects in infants in a triadic context: a pragmatic and semiotic perspective. Infant and Child Development, 24, 23-43. doi:10.1002/icd.1873

  • Perinat, A. (1993). Comunicación animal, comunicación humana [Animal communication, human communication]. Madrid: Siglo Veintiuno de España.

  • Piaget, J. (2007). El desarrollo de la inteligencia en el niño [Development of child’s intelligence]. Barcelona: Crítica.

  • Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C. (2006). Del ritmo al símbolo: Los signos en el nacimiento de la inteligencia [From rhythm to symbol: Signs in the birth of intelligence]. Barcelona: Horsori.

  • Rodríguez, C. (2012a). The functional permanence of the object: A product of consensus. In E. Martí & C. Rodríguez (Eds.), After Piaget (pp. 123–150). Brunswick: Transactions Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C. (2012b). El adulto como guía: ¿El eslabón perdido del desarrollo humano? [The adult as a guide: the missing link of human development?]. Padres y Maestros, 344, 23–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C., Moreno-Núñez, A., Sosa, N. & Basilio, M. (in revision). First shared reference with pointing gestures or with ostensive gestures? Some developmental implications. Monograph for Cognitive Development.

  • Rodríguez, C., & Moro, C. (1999). El mágico número tres. Cuando los niños aún no hablan. Barcelona: Paidós.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C., & Moro, C. (2008). Coming to agreement: Object use by infants and adults. In J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 89–114). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C., & Palacios, P. (2007). Do private gestures have a self-regulatory function?: A case study. Infant Behavior and Development, 30(2), 180–194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C., Palacios, P., Cárdenas, K., & Yuste, N. (2014). Les symboles: Formes de second ou de troisième sens? [The symbols: Forms of second or third sense?] In C. Moro & N. Müller-Mirza (Eds.), Psychologie du développement, sémiotique et culture (pp. 99-116) [Developmental psychology, semiosis and culture]. Villeneuve d’Ascq: Presses Universitaires du Septentrion.

  • Santiago, M. (2011). An embodied-socio-psychological perspective in health psychology? Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(5), 220–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinha, C., & Rodríguez, C. (2008). Language and the signifying object: From convention to imagination. In J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 357–378). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Southgate, V., Van Maanen, C., & Csibra, G. (2007). Infant pointing: communication to cooperate or communication to learn? Child Development, 78(3), 735–740.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2004). Learning through others. Daedalus Winter, 133(1), 51–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121–125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Hamann, K. (2012). The 37th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture Collaboration in young children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(1), 1–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trehub, S. (2003). The developmental origins of musicality. Nature Neuroscience, 7(6), 669–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1999). Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 1999–2000, 155–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (2003). Conversations with a two-month-old. Philadelfia: Whurr Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotski, L. S. (1984/1996). El primer año [The First Year]. In Vygotski, L. S., Obras escogidas IV. Psicología infantil (pp. 275–318). Madrid: Visor.

  • Wallon, H. (1972). La evolución psicológica del niño [The psychological evolution of the child]. Buenos Aires: Psique.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was granted by the Program “Formación de Profesorado Universitario” (Training for University Teachers), of the Ministry of Education, Spain, to what the first author belongs during the period 2009–2014. It was supported for the I + D + i project EDU2011-27840 as well, of the Ministry of Science and Innovation, Spain. We also thank the collaboration of Edgardo Miranda (Universidad de la Frontera, Chile), as a methodology consultant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ana Moreno-Núñez.

Additional information

Highlights:

We explore how early development of semiotic systems enable triadic interactions.

We propose that early triadic interactions have a communicative/educational nature.

Rhythm could be one of the first semiotic systems, and an instrument of communication.

Children easily respond to the adult’s rhythmic-sonorous proposals.

Adults favor rhythmic, sonorous and melodic components to interact with babies from 2 to 6 months.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Moreno-Núñez, A., Rodríguez, C. & Del Olmo, M.J. The Rhythmic, Sonorous and Melodic Components of Adult-Child-Object Interactions Between 2 and 6 Months Old. Integr. psych. behav. 49, 737–756 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-015-9298-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-015-9298-2

Keywords

Navigation