Skip to main content
Log in

Predicting individual differences in reading comprehension: a twin study

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined the Simple View of reading from a behavioral genetic perspective. Two aspects of word decoding (phonological decoding and word recognition), two aspects of oral language skill (listening comprehension and vocabulary), and reading comprehension were assessed in a twin sample at age 9. Using latent factor models, we found that overlap among phonological decoding, word recognition, listening comprehension, vocabulary, and reading comprehension was primarily due to genetic influences. Shared environmental influences accounted for associations among word recognition, listening comprehension, vocabulary, and reading comprehension. Independent of phonological decoding and word recognition, there was a separate genetic link between listening comprehension, vocabulary, and reading comprehension and a specific shared environmental link between vocabulary and reading comprehension. There were no residual genetic or environmental influences on reading comprehension. The findings provide evidence for a genetic basis to the “Simple View” of reading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aaron, P. G., Joshi, M., & Williams, K. A. (1999). Not all reading disabilities are alike. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 120–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and the AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Betjemann, R. S., Willcutt, E. G., Olson, R. K., Keenan, J. M., DeFries, J. C., & Wadsworth, S. J. (2008). Word reading and reading comprehension: Stability, overlap and independence. Reading and Writing, 21, 539–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braze, D., Tabor, W., Shankweiler, D. P., & Mencl, M. E. (2007). Speaking up for vocabulary: Reading skill differences in young adults. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 226–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B., Coventry, W. L., Olson, R. K., Samuelsson, S., Corley, R., Willcutt, E. G., et al. (2009). Genetic and environmental influences on aspects of literacy and language in early childhood: Continuity and change from preschool to Grade 2. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22, 219–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B., Samuelsson, S., Wadsworth, S., Hulslander, J., Corley, R., DeFries, J. C., et al. (2007). Longitudinal twin study of early literacy development: Preschool through Grade 1. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20, 77–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., & Weismer, S. E. (2006). Language deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 278–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Adlof, S. M. (2005). Developmental changes in reading and reading disabilities. In H. Catts & A. Kamhi (Eds.), The connections between language and reading disabilities (pp. 25–40). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., & Katch, E. L. (2004). Beyond the reading wars: The effect of classroom instruction by child interactions on early reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 305–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, L. E., & Scarborough, H. S. (2006). Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 277–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edmonds, M. S., Vaughn, S., Wexler, J., Reutebuch, C., Cable, A., Tackett, K. K., et al. (2009). A synthesis of reading interventions and effects on reading comprehension outcomes for older struggling readers. Review of Educational Research, 79, 262–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gayán, J., & Olson, R. K. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on individual differences in printed word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84, 97–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillam, R., & Pearson, N. (2004). Test of narrative language. Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (2001). The Boston Naming Test (2nd ed.). New York: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, E. A., & Morrison, F. J. (1997). The unique contribution of home literacy environment to differences in early literacy skills. Early Child Development & Care, 127–128, 233–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harlaar, N., Spinath, F. M., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2005). Genetic influences on early word recognition abilities and disabilities: A study of 7-year-old twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 373–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M., & Seidenberg, M. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in meaning: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111, 662–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayiou-Thomas, M. E. (2008). Genetic and environmental influences on early speech, language and literacy development. Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 397–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Harlaar, N., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2006). Genetic and environmental mediation of the prediction from preschool language and nonverbal ability to 7-year reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 50–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 55–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hohnen, B., & Stevenson, J. (1999). The structure of genetic influences on general cognitive, language, phonological, and reading abilities. Developmental Psychology, 35, 590–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading & Writing, 2, 127–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, F. B. (2009). Diet and lifestyle influences on risk of coronary heart disease. Current Atherosclerosis Reports, 11, 257–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Justice, L. M., McGinty, A. S., Cabell, S. Q., Kilday, C. R., Knighton, K., & Huffman, G. (2010). Language and literacy curriculum supplement for preschoolers who are academically at risk: A feasibility study. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 161–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R. S., & Olson, R. K. (2008). Reading comprehension tests vary in the skills they assess: Differential dependence on decoding and oral comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 281–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R. S., Wadsworth, S. J., DeFries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (2006). Genetic and environmental influences on reading and listening comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 79–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. S. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: The independent contributions of oral language and decoding skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 765–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendler, K. S., & Gardner, C. O. (1998). Twin studies of adult psychiatric and substance dependence disorders: Are they biased by differences in the environmental experiences of monozygotic and dizygotic twins in childhood and adolescence? Psychological Medicine, 28, 625–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kovas, Y., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Oliver, B., Dale, P. S., Bishop, D. V. M., & Plomin, R. (2005). Genetic influences in different aspects of language development: The etiology of language skills in 4.5-year-old twins. Child Development, 76, 632–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J. R., & Savage, R. S. (2008). Can the simple view deal with the complexities of reading? Literacy, 42, 75–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leach, J. M., Scarborough, H. S., & Rescorla, L. (2003). Late-emerging reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 211–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, B. A., Shriberg, L. D., Freebairn, L. A., Hansen, A. J., Stein, C. M., Taylor, H. G., et al. (2006). The genetic bases of speech sound disorders: Evidence from spoken and written language. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 1294–1312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J. C. (2004). Latent variable models: An introduction to factor, path, and structural equation analysis (4th ed.). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J., & Nichols, R. (1976). Heredity, environment and personality. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W., Borden, S. L., DeLuca, T., Lacerenza, L., Benson, M. J., & Brackston, D. (1994). Treating the core deficits of developmental dyslexia: Evidence of transfer of learning after phonologically- and strategy-based reading training programs. Developmental Psychology, 30, 805–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markwardt, F. C. (1989; 1997 Normative Update). Peabody Individual Achievement Test—revised. Bloomington: Pearson Assessments.

  • Mol, S. E., Bus, A. G., de Jong, M. T., & Smeets, D. J. H. (2008). Added value of dialogic parent–child book readings: A meta-analysis. Early Education and Development, 19, 7–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Angell, P. (2006). Learning to read and learning to comprehend. London Review of Education, 4, 77–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xi, G., & Maes, H. H. (2006). Mx: Statistical modeling (7th ed.). Richmond: Department of Psychiatry.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neale, M. C., & Cardon, L. R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, G. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: the role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 554–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, G., & Beers, A. (2009). A not-so-simple view of reading: How oral vocabulary and visual-word recognition complicate the story. Reading and Writing, 23, 189–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paracchini, S., Scerri, T., & Monaco, A. P. (2007). The genetic lexicon of dyslexia. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 8, 57–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbrow, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (Vol. 11, pp. 67–86). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrill, S. A., Deater-Deckard, K., Thompson, L. A., DeThorne, L. S., & Schatschneider, C. (2006). Reading skills in early readers: Genetic and shared environmental influences. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 48–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., & McGuffin, P. (2008). Behavioral genetics (5th ed.). New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R., & Kovas, Y. (2005). Generalist genes and learning disabilities. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 592–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price, T. S., Freeman, B., Craig, I., Petrill, S. A., Ebersole, L., & Plomin, R. (2000). Infant zygosity can be assigned by parental report questionnaire data. Twin Research, 3, 129–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. In P. V. Marsden (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 111–163). Cambridge: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld, M. J. H., Posthuma, D., Dolan, C. V., & Boomsma, D. I. (2003). ADHD: Sibling interaction or dominance: An evaluation of statistical power. Behavior Genetics, 33, 247–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarr, S., & Carter-Saltzman, L. (1979). Twin method: Defense of a critical assumption. Behavior Genetics, 9, 527–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E. M., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals—4. San Antonio: PsychCorp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Leikin, M. (2004). Language impairment at school entry and later reading disability: Connections at lexical versus supralexical levels of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 87–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., & Hayiou-Thomas, M. E. (2006). The dyslexia spectrum: Continuities between reading, speech, and language impairments. Topics in Language Disorders, 26, 108–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spiegelhalter, D. J., Best, N. G., Carlin, B. P., & van der Linde, A. (2002). Bayesian measures of model complexity and fit (with discussion). Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B Statistical Methodology, 64, 583–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Austin: Pro-ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torppa, M., Tolvanen, A., Poikkeus, A. M., Eklund, K., Lerkkanen, M. K., Leskinen, E., et al. (2007). Reading development subtypes and their early characteristics. Annals of Dyslexia, 57, 3–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • US Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Assessment of Educational Progress (2005). The NAEP reading achievement levels by grade. Retrieved from http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/reading/achieveall.asp.

  • Verhoeven, L., & van Leeuwe, J. (2008). Prediction of the development of Reading comprehension: A longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 407–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wade, T. D., Wilkinson, J., & Ben Tovim, D. (2003). The genetic epidemiology of body attitudes, the attitudinal component of body image in women. Psychological Medicine, 33, 1395–1405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, P. T., & Anderson, R. C. (1986). What they don’t know will hurt them: The role of prior knowledge in comprehension. In J. Orasanu (Ed.), Reading comprehension: From research to practice (pp. 31–48). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests, Form G—revised. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the ongoing contribution of the parents and children in the Western Research Reading Project (WRRP). WRRP is supported by NICHD grant HD38075 and NICHD/OSERS grant HD46167.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicole Harlaar.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Harlaar, N., Cutting, L., Deater-Deckard, K. et al. Predicting individual differences in reading comprehension: a twin study. Ann. of Dyslexia 60, 265–288 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0044-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0044-7

Keywords

Navigation