Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Perceptual organization, phonological awareness, and reading comprehension in adults with and without learning disabilities

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It is not clear from research whether, or to what extent, reading comprehension is impaired in adults who have learning disabilities (LD). The influence of perceptual organization (PO) and phonological awareness (PA) on reading comprehension was investigated. PO and PA are cognitive functions that have been examined in previous research for their roles in nonverbal LD and phonological dyslexia, respectively. Nonverbal tests of PO and non-reading tests of PA were administered to a sample of adults with postsecondary education. Approximately two thirds of the sample had previously been diagnosed as having LD. In a multiple regression analysis, tests of PO and PA were used to predict scores for tests of reading comprehension and mechanics. Despite the nonverbal nature of the perceptual organizational test stimuli, PO strongly predicted reading comprehension. Tests of PA predicted decoding and reading speed. Results were interpreted as supporting the hypothesis that integrative processes usually characterized as nonverbal were nonetheless used by readers with and without disabilities to understand text. The study’s findings have implications for understanding the reading of adults with learning disabilities, and the nature of reading comprehension in general.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Specific information concerning the influence of re-administration of the WAIS on subtest scores in the longer term is lacking in typical adults, and in those with LD (Groth-Marnat, 2009). As a consequence, clinical standards concerning the necessity for current information versus practice effects and the stability of adult cognitive profiles (Association on Higher Education and Disability, 2009; Learning Disabilities Association of Ontario, 2003) guided the decision not to re-administer Block Design or Vocabulary when scores were less than 3 years old.

References

  • Acheson, D. J., Wells, J. B., & MacDonald, M. C. (2008). New and updated tests of print exposure and reading abilities in college students. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 278–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Agnew, J. A., Dorn, C., & Eden, G. F. (2004). Effect of intensive training on auditory processing and reading skills. Brain and Language, 88, 21–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atchley, R. A., & Atchley, P. (1998). Hemispheric specialization in the detection of subjective objects. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1373–1386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atchley, R. A., Story, J., & Buchanan, L. (2001). Exploring the contribution of the cerebral hemispheres to language comprehension deficits in adults with developmental language disorder. Brain and Cognition, 46, 16–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beeman, M. (1994). Semantic processing in the right hemisphere may contribute to drawing inferences from discourse. Brain and Language, 44, 80–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beeman, M. J. (1998). Coarse semantic coding and discourse comprehension. In M. Beeman & C. Chiarello (Eds.), Right hemisphere language comprehension: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience (pp. 255–284). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann, M., & Kimchi, R. (2003). What does visual agnosia tell us about perceptual organization and its relationship to object perception? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 19–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, N. (1991). Gestalt imagery: A critical factor in language comprehension. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 246–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, L. C., & Perfetti, C. A. (1994). Reading skill: Some adult comparisons. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 244–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Dror, I., Pollatsek, A., & Scarpati, S. (1991). Word identification in isolation and in context by college dyslexic students. Brain and Language, 40, 471–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benowitz, L. I., Moya, K. L., & Levine, D. N. (1990). Impaired verbal reasoning and constructional apraxia in subjects with right hemisphere damage. Neuropsychologia, 28, 231–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birch, S., & Chase, C. (2004). Visual and language processing deficits in compensated and uncompensated college students with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 389–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bone, B., Cirino, P., Morris, M., & Morris, R. (2002). Reading and phonological awareness in reading disabled adults. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 305–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Normative data for 144 compound remote associate problems. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 634–639.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braze, D., Tabor, W., Shankweiler, D. P., & Mencl, E. W. (2007). Speaking up for vocabulary: Reading skill differences in young adults. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 226–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. I., Fishco, V. V., & Hanna, G. (1993). Nelson–Denny reading test, forms G & H. Itasca: Riverside.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, V. L., Hammill, D. D., & Wiederholt, J. L. (1986). Test of reading comprehension (TORC) (3rd ed.). Scarborough: Nelson Thomson Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, H. H., Simpson, T. L., Bihrle, A. M., Potter, H. H., & Gardner, H. (1990). Appreciation of metaphoric alternative word meanings by left and right brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychologia, 28, 375–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 439–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunswick, N., McCrory, E., Price, C. J., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics: A search for Wernicke's Wortschatz? Brain, 122, 1091–1917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, B., Patton, J., & Dunn, C. (1991). Scholastic abilities test for adults (SATA). Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chabris, C. F., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1998). How do the cerebral hemispheres contribute to encoding spatial relations? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 8–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, L. K. S., Cole, P. G., & Morris, J. N. (1990). Effects of instruction in the use of a visual–imagery strategy on the reading comprehension competence of disabled and average readers. Learning Disability Quarterly, 13, 2–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cipolotti, L., Robinson, G., Blair, J., & Frith, U. (1999). Fractionation of visual memory: Evidence from a case with multiple neurodevelopmental impairments. Neuropsychologia, 37, 455–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental rotation and the right hemisphere. Brain and Language, 57, 100–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corkett, J. K., & Parrila, R. (2007). Use of context in the word recognition process by adults with a significant history of reading difficulties. Annals of Dyslexia, 58, 139–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornoldi, C., Venneri, A., Marconato, F., Molin, A., & Montinari, C. (2003). A rapid screening measure for the identification of visuospatial learning disability in schools. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 299–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., Stanovich, K. E., & Wilson, M. R. (1990). Cognitive variation in adult college students differing in reading ability. In T. H. Carr & B. A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 129–180). San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, L. E., & Scarborough, H. S. (2006). Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 277–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, A. (2008). Reading fluency in adults. In L. E. Wolf, H. E. Schreiber, & J. Wasserstein (Eds.), Adult learning disabilities: Contemporary issues (pp. 111–126). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, S. H., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. R. (2006). Processing of derived forms in high functioning dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 103–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dien, J. (2009). A tale of two recognition systems: Implications of the fusiform face area and the visual word form area for lateralized object recognition models. Neuropsychologia, 47, 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Association on Higher Education and Disability (2009). Best practices resources. http://www.ahead.org/resources/best-practices-resources

  • Dool, C. B., Stelmack, R. M., & Rourke, B. P. (1993). Event-related potentials in children with learning disabilities. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 387–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faust, M., & Kahana, A. (2002). Priming summation in the cerebral hemispheres: Evidence from semantically convergent and semantically divergent primes. Neuropsychologia, 40, 892–901.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferstl, E. C., Neumann, J., Bogler, C., & von Cramon, D. (2008). The extended language network: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 29, 581–593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forrest, B. J. (2004). The utility of math difficulties, internalized psychopathology, and visual–spatial deficits to identify children with nonverbal learning disability syndrome: Evidence for a visual–spatial disability. Child Neuropsychology, 10(2), 129–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foss, J. M. (1991). Nonverbal learning disabilities and remedial interventions. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 128–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The reading span task and its predictive power for reading comprehension ability. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 136–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuerst, D. R., Fisk, J. L., & Rourke, B. P. (1989). Psychosocial functioning of learning disabled children: Replicability of statistically derived subtypes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 275–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. M. (1993). Neuroanatomic basis of developmental dyslexia. Neurologic Clinics, 11, 161–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garnham, A., & Oakhill, J. (1996). The mental models theory of language comprehension. In B. K. Britton & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Models of understanding text (pp. 313–340). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garson, G. D. (2009). Reliability analysis, from Statnotes: Topics in multivariate analysis. Retrieved 11/28/2009 from http://faculty.chass.ncsu.edu/garson/pa765/statnote.htm.

  • Goldberg, E., & Costa, L. D. (1981). Hemisphere differences in the acquisition and use of descriptive systems. Brain and Language, 14, 144–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A., Siegel, L. S., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). The assessment of adults with reading disabilities: What can we learn from experimental tasks? Journal of Research in Reading, 20, 42–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, A., Wilson, A. M., & Gottardo, A. (2007). The role of print exposure in reading skills of postsecondary students with and without reading disabilities. Exceptionality Education Canada, 17, 175–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross-Tsur, V., Shalev, R. S., Manor, O., & Amir, N. (1995). Developmental right-hemisphere syndrome: Clinical spectrum of the nonverbal learning disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 80–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Groth-Marnat, G. (2009). Handbook of psychological assessment (5th ed.). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatcher, J., Snowling, M., & Griffiths, Y. M. (2002). Cognitive assessment of dyslexic students in higher education. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 119–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henson, R., Burgess, N., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Recoding, storage, rehearsal and grouping in verbal short-term memory: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 38, 426–440.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommet, C., Vidal, J., Roux, S., Blanc, R., Barthez, M. A., De Becque, B., et al. (2009). Topography of syllable change-detection electrophysiological indices in children and adults with reading disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 47, 761–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoskyn, M., & Swanson, H. L. (2000). Cognitive processing of low achievers and children with reading disabilities: a selective meta-analytic review of the published literature. School Psychology Review, 29, 102–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., Maughan, S., & Brown, G. D. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 685–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphries, T., Oram Cardy, J., Worling, D. E., & Peets, K. (2004). Narrative comprehension and retelling abilities of children with nonverbal learning disabilities. Brain and Cognition, 56, 77–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, A. M., Barnes, M. A., & Desrochers, A. (2008). Reading comprehension: Developmental processes, individual differences, and interventions. Canadian Psychology, 49, 125–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. W., Branigan, H. P., & Kelly, M. L. (2007). Visual deficits in developmental dyslexia: Relationships between non-linguistic visual tasks and their contribution to components of reading. Dyslexia, 14, 95–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., Carpenter, P. A., & Keller, T. A. (1996). The capacity theory of comprehension: New frontiers of evidence and arguments. Psychological Review, 103, 773–780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L. J., Goldstein, G., & Beers, S. R. (2001). Learning disabilities in older adolescents and adults: Clinical utility of the neuropsychological perspective. NY: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1994). K-SNAP: Kaufman short neuropsychological assessment procedure. Circle Pines: AGS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimchi, R. (1992). Primacy of wholistic processing and global/local paradigm: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 24–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J. R. (2007). Higher education students with reading and writing difficulties. Exceptionality EducationCanada, 17, 129–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J. R., Silvestri, R., Allingham, B. H., Parrila, R., & LaFave, C. B. (2008). Learning strategies and study approaches of postsecondary students with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 85–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Learning Disabilities Association of Ontario. (2003). Recommended practices for assessment, diagnosis, and documentation of learning disabilities. http://www.ldao.ca/documents/Assessment%20Protocols_Sept%2003.pdf

  • Lefly, D., & Pennington, B. (1991). Spelling errors and reading fluency in compensated dyslexics. Annals of Dyslexia, 41, 143–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N. K., Pearson, M. R., & Siegel, L. S. (2006). The effects of timed and untimed testing conditions on the reading comprehension performance of adults with reading disabilities. Reading and Writing, 19, 21–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liddell, G. A., & Rasmussen, C. (2005). Memory profile of children with nonverbal learning disability. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 20, 137–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindell, A. K. (2006). In your right mind: Right hemisphere contributions to language processing and production. Neuropsychological Review, 16, 131–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mamen, M. (2007). Understanding nonverbal learning disabilities: A common-sense guide for parents and professionals. London: Jessica Kingsley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mapou, R. L. (2004). Assessment of learning disabilities. In J. H. Ricker (Ed.), Differential diagnosis in adult neuropsychological assessment (pp. 370–420). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, R. A., & Just, M. A. (2004). How the brain processes causal inferences in text: A theoretical account of generation and integration component processes utilizing both cerebral hemispheres. Psychological Science, 15, 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T. E., & Graetz, J. E. (2003). Reading comprehension instruction for secondary students: Challenges for struggling students and teachers. Learning Disability Quarterly, 26, 103–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mattson, A. J., Sheer, D. E., & Fletcher, J. M. (1992). Electrophysiological evidence of lateralized disturbances in children with learning disabilities. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 707–716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCallum, R. D., & Moore, S. (1999). Not all imagery is created equal: The role of imagery in the comprehension of main ideas in exposition. Journal of Reading Psychology, 20, 21–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller-Shaul, S. (2005). The characteristics of young and adult dyslexic readers on reading and reading related cognitive tasks as compared to normal readers. Dyslexia, 11, 132–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mosberg, L., & Johns, D. (1994). Reading and listening comprehension in college students with developmental dyslexia. Learning Disabilities Research, 9, 130–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moss Thompson, O. (1985). The nonverbal dilemma. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 18, 400–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myklebust, H. E. (1975). Nonverbal learning disabilities: Assessment and intervention. In H. E. Myklebust (Ed.), Progress in learning disabilities (pp. 85–120). New York: Grune-Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naglieri, J. A., & Kaufman, A. S. (2001). Using the cognitive assessment system (CAS) with learning-disabled children. In J. A. Naglieri, A. S. Kaufman, & N. L. Kaufman (Eds.), Specific learning disabilities and difficulties in children and adolescents: Psychological assessment and evaluation (pp. 141–177). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nichelli, P., & Venneri, A. (1995). Right hemisphere developmental learning disability: A case study. Neurocase, 1, 173–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nippold, M. A., & Duthie, J. K. (2003). Mental imagery and idiom comprehension: A comparison of school-age children and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 788–799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 443–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J., & Patel, S. (1991). Can imagery training help children who have comprehension problems? Journal of Research in Reading, 14, 106–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ofiesh, N., Mather, N., & Russell, A. (2005). Using speeded cognitive, reading, and academic measures to determine the need for extended time among university students with learning disabilities. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 23, 35–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., Forsberg, H., Wise, B., & Rack, J. (1994). Measurement of word recognition, orthographic, and phonological skills. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 243–278). Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (2005). Looking at reading comprehension through the lens of neuroscience. In C. Collins Block & S. R. Parris (Eds.), Comprehension instruction: Research-based best practices (pp. 101–113). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palombo, J. (1993). Neurocognitive deficits, developmental distortions, and incoherent narratives. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 13, 85–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, D. R., & Benedict, K. B. (2002). Assessment and intervention: Promoting successful transition for college students with ADHD. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 27, 3–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulesu, E., Frith, U., Snowling, M., Gallagher, A., Morton, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., et al. (1996). Is developmental dyslexia a disconnection syndrome? Evidence from PET scanning. Brain, 119, 143–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelletier, P. M., Ahmad, S. A., & Rourke, B. P. (2001). Classification rules for basic phonological processing disabilities and nonverbal learning disabilities: Formulation and external validity. Child Neuropsychology, 7, 84–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F. (2009). Diagnosing learning disorders: A neuropsychological framework (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., van Orden, G. C., Smith, S. D., Green, P. A., & Haith, M. M. (1990). Phonological processing skills and deficits in adult dyslexics. Child Development, 61, 1753–1778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., et al. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841–865.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ransby, M. J., & Swanson, H. L. (2003). Reading comprehension skills of young adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 538–555.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, T. L. (2001). Functional magnetic resonance imaging and spectroscopic imaging of the brain: Application of fMRI and fMRS to reading disabilities and education. Learning Disability Quarterly, 24, 189–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rourke, B. P. (1995). Syndrome of nonverbal learning disabilities: Neurodevelopmental manifestations. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rourke, B. P. (2000). Neuropsychological and psychosocial subtyping: a review of investigations within the University of Windsor laboratory. Canadian Psychology, 41, 34–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rourke, B. P., & Tsatsanis, K. D. (1996). Syndrome of nonverbal learning disabilities: Psycholinguistic assets and deficits. Topics in Language Disorders, 16, 30–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sabatini, J. P. (2002). Efficiency in word reading of adults: Ability group comparisons. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 267–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2001). Imagery and text: A dual coding theory of reading and writing. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S., & Brady, S. A. (2002). Toward a common terminology for talking about speech and reading: A glossary of the “phon” words and some related terms. Journal of Literacy Research, 34, 299–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shafrir, U., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Subtypes of learning disabilities in adolescents and adults. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 123–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S., & Shaywitz, B. (2005). Dyslexia (specific reading disability). Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1301–1309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Fullbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., Mencl, W. E., Constable, R. T., et al. (2003). Neural systems for compensation and persistence: Young adult outcome of childhood reading disability. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 25–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, F., & Singleton, C. (2000). The reading comprehension abilities of dyslexic students in higher education. Dyslexia, 6, 178–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Bergman, E., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2000). Cerebral mechanisms involved in word reading in dyslexic children: A magnetic source imaging approach. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 809–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., Nation, K., Moxham, P., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (1997). Phonological processing skills of dyslexic students in higher education: A preliminary report. Journal of Research in Reading, 20, 31–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • St George, M., Kutas, M., Martinez, A., & Sereno, M. (1999). Semantic integration in reading: Engagement of the right hemisphere in discourse processing. Brain, 122, 1317–1325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, J., Talcott, J., & Walsh, V. (2000). Controversy about the visual magnocellular deficit in developmental dyslexics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 209–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuebing, K. K., Fletcher, J. M., LeDoux, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2002). Validity of IQ-discrepancy classifications of reading disabilities: a meta-analysis. American Educational Research Journal, 39, 469–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Jerman, O. (2007). The influence of working memory on reading growth in subgroups of children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 249–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Temple, E. (2002). Brain mechanisms in normal and dyslexic readers. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12, 178–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tractenberg, R. (2002). Exploring hypotheses about phonological awareness, memory, and reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 407–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsatsanis, K. D., & Rourke, B. P. (2003). Syndrome of nonverbal learning disabilities: Effects on learning. In A. H. Fine & R. A. Kotkin (Eds.), Therapist’s guide to learning and attention disorders (pp. 109–145). San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. (2006). Gestalts of thought. In L. Albertazzi (Ed.), Visual thought: Advances in consciousness research (pp. 155–163). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Virtue, S., Parrish, T., & Jung-Beeman, M. J. (2008). Inferences during story comprehension: Cortical recruitment affected by predictability of events and working memory capacity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 2274–2284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voeller, K. K. S. (1986). Right-hemisphere deficit syndrome in children. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 143, 1004–1009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volden, J. (2004). Nonverbal learning disability: A tutorial for speech-language pathologists. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 13, 128–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Von Károlyi, C., Winner, E., Gray, W., & Sherman, G. F. (2003). Dyslexia linked to talent: Global visual spatial ability. Brain and Language, 85, 427–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vukovic, R. K., & Siegel, L. (2006). The double-deficit hypothesis: A comprehensive analysis of the evidence. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 25–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, B. J., Truscott, D. M., Gambrell, L. B., & Almasi, J. (1994). Mental imagery, text illustrations, and reading comprehension of adult readers. In E. G. Sturtevant, W. M. Linke, K. A. Mohr, & E. W. Murphy (Eds.), Pathways for literacy: Learners teach and teachers learn: the sixteenth yearbook of the college reading association (pp. 99–108). Pittsburg: College Reading.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wapner, W., Hamby, S., & Gardner, H. (1981). The role of the right hemisphere in the apprehension of complex linguistic materials. Brain and Language, 14, 15–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (1996). The measurement of verbal working memory capacity and its relation to reading comprehension. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 51–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weaver, S. M. (1993). The validity of the use of extended and untimed testing for postsecondary students with learning disabilities. Dissertation Abstracts International, 55 (03), (University Microfilms No. AAT NN863341.)

  • Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler adult intelligence scale—Third edition: Administration and scoring manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D., Coalson, D. L., & Raiford, S. E. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale—Fourth edition: Technical and interpretive manual. San Antonio: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weintraub, S., & Mesulam, M.-M. (1983). Developmental learning disabilities of the right hemisphere: emotional, interpersonal, and cognitive components. Archives of Neurology, 40, 463–468.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (1993). The incidental acquisition of information from reading. Psychological Science, 2, 325–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, A. M., & Lesaux, N. K. (2001). Persistence of phonological processing deficits in college students with dyslexia who have age-appropriate reading skills. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 394–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). The Woodcock–Johnson tests of cognitive abilities and academic achievement (3rd ed.). Scarborough: Nelson Thomson Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Worling, D. E., Humphries, T., & Tannock, R. (1999). Spatial and emotional aspects of language inferencing in nonverbal learning disabilities. Brain and Language, 70, 220–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zatorre, R. J. (2003). Functional and structural imaging in the study of auditory language processes. In M. T. Banich & M. Mack (Eds.), Mind, brain, and language: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 211–228). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeffiro, T., & Eden, G. (2000). The neural basis of developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 3–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, J., Tulsky, D. S., Price, L., & Chen, H.-Y. (2001). WAIS-III reliability data for clinical groups. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 862–866.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Margot Stothers.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Stothers, M., Klein, P.D. Perceptual organization, phonological awareness, and reading comprehension in adults with and without learning disabilities. Ann. of Dyslexia 60, 209–237 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0042-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0042-9

Keywords

Navigation