Skip to main content
Log in

Reading fluency: The whole is more than the parts

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the relative contributions of phonological awareness, orthographic pattern recognition, and rapid letter naming to fluent word and connected-text reading within a dyslexic sample of 123 children in second and third grades. Participants were assessed on a variety of fluency measures and reading subskills. Correlations and hierarchical multiple regression analyses were carried out to explore these relationships. The results demonstrate that phonological awareness, rapid letter naming, and orthographic pattern recognition contribute to word-reading skills. Furthermore, rapid naming, orthographic pattern recognition, and word reading fluency moderately predict different dimensions of connected-text reading (i.e., rate, accuracy, and comprehension) whereas phonological awareness contributes only to the comprehension dimension of connected-text reading. The findings support the multidimensional nature of fluency in which the whole is more than its parts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allington, R. L. (1983). Fluency: The neglected reading goal in reading instruction. The Reading Teacher, 36, 556–561.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. (2005). Does a visual-orthographic deficit contribute to reading disability? Annals of Dyslexia, 55, 28–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. (2001). Process assessment of the learner (PAL) test battery for reading and writing. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Billingsley, F., & Nagy, W. (2001). Processes underlying timing and fluency of reading: Efficiency, automaticity, coordination, and morphological awareness. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Time, fluency, and dyslexia (pp. 383–415). Parkton, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourassa, D., & Treiman, R. (2003). Spelling in children with dyslexia: Analyses from the Treiman-Bourassa early spelling test. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 309–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G. (1993). Text reading and rereading: Determinants of fluency beyond word recognition. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25(2), 133–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P., Sunseth, K., & Golden, J. (1999). The route between rapid naming and reading progress. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 31–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (1997). Effects of accelerated reading rate on memory for text among dyslexic readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 289–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (2001). The determinants of reading fluency: A comparison of dyslexic and average. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Time, fluency, and dyslexia (pp. 245–277). Parkton, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., & Share, D. L. (1992). Effects of accelerated reading on memory of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 193–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • British Psychological Society. (1999). Dyslexia, literacy and psychological assessment. Leicester, UK: British Psychological Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A., & Miozzo, M. (1997). The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: Evidence from the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon. Cognition, 64, 309–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, T. H. (2001). Trying to understand reading and dyslexia: Mental chronometry, individual differences, cognitive neuroscience, and the impact of instruction as converging sources of evidence. In R. Klein & P. McMullen (Eds.), Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia (pp. 459–491). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornwall, A. (1992). The relationship of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and verbal memory to severe reading and spelling disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 532–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronin, V., & Carver, P. (1998). Phonological sensitivity, rapid naming, and beginning reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 447–461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid automatized naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doehring, D. G. (1976). Acquisition of rapid reading responses. Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development, 41(2), Serial No. 165.

  • Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1983). Development of word identification speed in skilled and less skilled beginning readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 3–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, G. B., Griffin, J. R., Bowman, B., Hinkle, J., Grove, J., & Kaw, R. (2000). Dyseidetic dyslexia. Journal of Optometric Vision Development, 31, 29–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, M. E., & Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 460–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1994). Attention as a process and as a disorder. In G.R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 103–116). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Steubing, K., Shaywitz, B. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (1994). Measurement of change: Assessing behavior over time and within a developmental framework. In G. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 29–58). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Good, R. H., & Kaminski, R. A. (2002). DIBELS oral reading and fluency passages for first through third grades (Technical Report No. 10). Eugene, OR: University of Oregon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P., Juel, C., & Griffith, P. (1992). Reading, spelling, and the orthographic cipher. In P. Gough, L. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 35–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, C., Mulhern, G., & Neil, P. (2000). Automaticity, spelling, and second language learning. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 90, 890–892.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., Broek, P., Espin, C., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Accuracy and fluency in list and context reading of skilled and RD groups: Absolute and relative performance levels. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 18(4), 237–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kame’enui, E. J., & Simmons, D. C. (2001). The DNA of reading fluency. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 203–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kame’enui, E. J., Simmons, D. C., Good, R. H., & Harn, B. A. (2000). The use of fluency-based measures in early identification and evaluation of intervention efficacy in schools. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Time, fluency, and dyslexia (pp. 307–333). Parkton, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, T. (2002). Multiple pathways to dysfluent reading: A developmental-componential investigation of the development and breakdown of fluent reading. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Tufts University, Medford, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, T., Kim, Y., Wolf, M., Kennedy, B., Morris, R., & Lovett, M. (in press). The relationship of spelling recognition, RAN, and phonological awareness to reading skills in older poor readers and younger reading-matched controls. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal.

  • Katzir, T., Misra, M., & Poldrack, R. (2005). Imaging phonology without print: Assessing the neural correlates of phonemic awareness using fMRI. Neuroimage, 27, 106–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (1990). K-BIT: Kaufman brief intelligence test. Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B. A., & Carr, T. H. (1990). Component process analyses: Conclusion and challenges. In T. H. Carr & B. A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 423–438). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1997). Automaticity and reading: Perspectives from the instance theory of automation. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 13, 146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W. (1984). A developmental perspective on reading dysfunction: Accuracy and rate criteria in the subtyping of dyslexic children. Brain and Language, 22, 67–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W. (1987). A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed criteria of normal and deficient reading skill. Child Development, 58, 234–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, G. R., & Moats, L. C. (1997). Critical conceptual and methodological considerations in reading intervention research. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 578–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speech, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 325–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markwardt, F. C., Jr. (1989). Peabody individual achievement test (PIAT). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, M. S., & Felton, R. H. (1999). Repeated reading to enhance fluency: Old approaches and new directions. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 283–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Misra, M., Katzir, T., Wolf, M., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Neural systems for rapid automatized naming (RAN) in skilled readers: Unraveling the puzzle of the RAN-reading relationship. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 241–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (1997). Assessing reading difficulties: The validity and utility of current measures of reading skill. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 359–370.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rupley, W. H. (2005). Introduction to special issue. Reading Psychology, 26, 103–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, C., Carlson, C. D., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., & Fletcher, J. M. (2002). Relationship of rapid automatized naming and phonological awareness in early reading development: Implications for the double-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 245–256.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, C., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M., & Mehta, P. (1999). The dimensionality of phonological awareness: An application of item response theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 439–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B., Fletcher, J., & Escobar, M. (1990). Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls. Journal of the American Medical Association, 264, 998–1002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D., & Levin, I. (1999). Learning to read and write in Hebrew. In M. Harris & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 89–111). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C., Griffin., P., & Burns, S. (2005). Knowledge to support the teaching of reading: Preparing teachers for a changing world. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, L., & Downey, D. (1995). Serial rapid naming skills in children with reading disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 31–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Speece, D. L., Mills, C., Ritchey, K. D., & Hillman, E. (2003). Initial evidence that letter fluency tasks are valid indicators of early reading skill. The Journal of Special Education, 36(4), 223–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K., & Siegel, L. A. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P., Miller, S., & Fitch, R. H. (1993). Neurobiological basis of speech: A case of the preeminence of temporal processing. In P. Tallal, A. M. Galaburda, R. R. Llinas, & C. V. Euler (Eds.), Temporal information processing in the nervous system (pp. 421–423). New York: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Alexander, A. (2001). Principles of fluency instruction in reading: Relationships with established empirical outcomes. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 333–357). Parkton, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., Rashotte, C., Lindamood, P., Rose, E., Conway, T., & Garven, C. (1999). Preventing reading failure in young children with phonological awareness disabilities: Group and individual responses to instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 579–593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency (TOWRE). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences. (2005). National assessment of educational progress: The nation’s report card, Reading 2005. (NCES Publication No. 2006-451). Washington, DC: Perie, M., Grigg, W.S., & Donahue, P.L.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Leij, A., & van Daal, H. P. (1999). Automatization aspects of dyslexia: Speed limitations in word identification, sensitivity to increasing task demands, and orthographic compensation. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 417–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R. (2003). Individual differences as sources of variability in reading comprehension in elementary school children. In A. P. Sweet & C. E. Snow (Eds.), Rethinking reading comprehension (pp. 51–81). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological awareness (CTOPP). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiederholt, J. L., & Bryant, B. R. (1992). Gray oral reading tests (GORT). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, G. S. (1993). Wide range achievement test (WRAT). Wilmington, DE: Wide Range, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bally, H., & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes, and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development, 57, 988–1000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. (1999). The “double-deficit hypothesis” for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Denckla, M. B. (2005). Rapid automatized naming and rapid alternating stimulus tests (RAN/RAS). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Katzir- Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 211–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Katzir-Cohen, T., Cirino, P., O’Brien, B., Morris, R., & Lovett, M. (2001, June). Fluency, phonology, and naming-speed in subtypes of dyslexia. Paper presented at the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Boulder, CO.

  • Wolf, M., O’Rourke, A. G., Gidney, C., Lovett, M. W., Cirino, P., & Morris, R. (2002). The second deficit: An investigation of the independence of phonological and naming-speed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 43–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, F., Flowers, L., & Grigorenko, E. (2001). On the functional neuroanatomy of fluency or why walking is just as important to reading as talking is. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Time, fluency, and dyslexia (pp. 235–245). Parkton, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery test-revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, A., & Bowers, P. G. (1995). Individual differences and text difficulty determinants of reading fluency and expressiveness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 428–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tami Katzir.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Katzir, T., Kim, Y., Wolf, M. et al. Reading fluency: The whole is more than the parts. Ann. of Dyslexia 56, 51–82 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-006-0003-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-006-0003-5

Key Words

Navigation