Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Does a visual-orthographic deficit contribute to reading disability?

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this study, visual-orthographic skills were defined as the ability to recognize whether letters and numerals are correctly oriented. Aims were to investigate whether visual-orthographic skills would contribute independent variance to reading, and whether children with a visual-orthographic deficit would be more impaired readers than similar children without this deficit. Participants were 207 children, aged 8 to 10 years, who attended school in a small suburban community. Because of the evidence that phonological awareness and naming speed are strongly related to reading, visual-orthographic skills were entered into hierarchical regression analyses following these variables. With age, verbal IQ, and verbal short-term memory also controlled, visual-orthographic skills accounted for significant independent variance in all reading measures. When children with a visual—orthographic deficit (29% of the sample) were compared with those without this deficit, they were significantly lower on all reading variables. At 8 to 10 years of age, reading progress of some children continues to be hampered by a problem in orthographic memory for the orientation of letters and numerals. Such children will require special attention, but their problems may be overlooked. As recommended by Willows and Terepocki (1993), there is need for further research on the phenomenon of letter reversals when they occur among children beyond first grade.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M. J., & Bruck, M. (1993). Word recognition: The interface of educational policies and scientific research. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 113–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A. (1993a). Phonemic awareness, visual symbol processing, and reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 87–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A. (1993b). Predicting reading progress in children receiving special help. Annals of Dyslexia, 43, 90–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A. (1994). Do dyslexic and other poor readers differ in reading-related cognitive skills? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 45–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A. (1996). Dyslexia: A validation of the concept at two age levels. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 102–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A. (1997). Dyslexia and the double deficit hypothesis. Annals of Dyslexia, 47, 69–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, C. E., Manis, F. R., Pedersen, W. C., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2004). Variation among developmental dyslexics: Evidence from a printed-word-learning task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 125–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, T. A., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (1992). The role of orthographic processing skills on five different reading tasks. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 334–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boden, C., & Brodeur, D. A. (1999). Visual processing of verbal and nonverbal stimuli in adolescents with reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 58–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. R., Perfetti, C. A., MacWhinney, B., & Hunt, S. B. (2000). The association of rapid temporal processing with orthographic and phonological processing in children and adults with reading impairment. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 101–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., & Ishaik, G. (2003). RAN’s contributions to understanding reading disabilities. In H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp.140–157). New York: Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S. A., & Shankweiler, D. P. (1991). Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cairns, N. U., & Steward, M. S. (1970). Young children’s orientation of letters as a function of axis of symmetry and stimulus alignment. Child Development, 41, 993–1002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chase, C. (1996). A visual deficit model of developmental dyslexia. In C. Chase, G. Rosen, & G. Sherman (Eds.), Developmental dyslexia: Neurological, cognitive, and genetic mechanisms (pp. 127–156). Baltimore: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornelissen, P. L., & Hansen, P. C. (1998). Motion detection, letter position encoding, and single word reading. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 155–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornwall, A. (1992). The relationship of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and verbal memory to severe reading and spelling disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 532–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., Perry, K. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2001). Converging evidence for the concept of orthographic processing. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 549–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtin, S., Manis, F. R., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). Parallels between the reading and spelling deficits of two groups of developmental dyslexics. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 515–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. (1974). Rapid “automatized naming” of pictured objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eden, G. F., Stein, J. F., Wood, M. H., & Wood, F. B. (1995). Verbal and visual problems in reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 272–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1985). Movement into reading: Is the first stage of printed word learning visual or phonetic? Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 163–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M., & Satz, P. (1979). Unitary deficit hypotheses of reading disabilities: Has Vellutino led us astray? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 12, 155–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R. (1994). Phonological and orthographic processing: Separate but equal? In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge, I: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 321–357). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gustafson, S. (2001). Cognitive abilities and print exposure in surface and phonological types of reading disability. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 351–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinshelwood, J. (1895). Word-blindness and visual memory. Lancet, 2, 1564–1570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iles, J., Walsh, V., & Richardson, A. (2000). Visual search performance in dyslexia. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 6, 163–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, N. E., & Coltheart, M. (2001). Routes to reading success and failure: Toward an integrated psychology of atypical reading. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, B. T. (1980 & 1990). Jordan left-right reversal test. Novato, CA: Academic Therapy Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lachmann, T. (2002). Reading disability as a deficit in functional coordination. In E. Witruk, A. D. Friederici, & T. Lachmann (Eds.), Basic functions of language, reading, and reading disability (pp. 165–198). Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 325–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M., McBride-Chang, C., & Pedersen, A. (1996). On the basis of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Stallings, L., Joanisse, M., Bailey, C., Freedman, L., & Curtin, S. (1999). Development of dyslexic subgroups: A one-year follow up. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 105–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Szeszulski, P. A., Holt, L. K., & Graves, L. (1990). Variation in component recognition and spelling skills among dyslexic children and normal readers. In T. H. Carr & B. A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 207–259). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Manis, F. R. (1996). Structural invariance in the associations of naming speed, phonological awareness, and verbal reasoning in good and poor readers: A test of the double deficit hypothesis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 323–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, M. S., Wood, F. B., Hart, L. A., & Felton, R. H. (1998). Longitudinal course of rapid naming in disabled and nondisabled readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 91–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, W. P. (1896). A case of congenital word-blindness. British Medical Journal, 2, 1612–1614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., & Datta, H. (2002). Visual-temporal processing in reading-disabled and normal twins. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 127–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. K., Forsberg, H., & Wise, B. (1994). Genes, environment, and the development of orthographic skills. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 27–71). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Psychological Corporation. (1992). Wechsler individual achievement test. San Antonio, TX: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J. P., & Olson, R. K. (1993). Phonological deficits, IQ, and individual differences in reading disability: Genetic and environmental influences. Developmental Review, 13, 269–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosner, J. (1979). Helping children overcome learning difficulties. New York: Walker & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, C., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M., & Mehta, P. (1999). The dimensionality of phonological awareness: An application of item response theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 439–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., & Evans, H. M. (1993). The visual (orthographic) processor and developmental dyslexia. In D. M. Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processes in reading and reading disabilities (pp. 347–376). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early reading acquisition. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 307–342). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., Siegel, L. S., & Gottardo, A. (1997). Converging evidence for phonological and surface subtypes of reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 114–127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, J. (2001a). The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 7, 12–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, J. (2001b). The neurobiology of reading difficulties. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 3–21). Timonium, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stein, J., Talcott, J., and Witton, C. (2001). The sensorimotor basis of developmental dyslexia. In A. Fawcett (Ed.), Dyslexia: Theory and good practice (pp. 63–88). London: Whurr Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talcott, J. B., Witton, C., Hebb, G. S., Stoodley, C. J., Westwood, F. A., France, S. J., Hansen, P. C., & Stein, J. F. (2002). On the relationship between dynamic visual and auditory processing and literacy skills: Results from a large primary-school study. Dyslexia: An International Journal of Research and Practice, 8, 204–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terepocki, M., Kruk, R. S., & Willows, D. M. (2002). The incidence and nature of letter orientation errors in reading disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 214–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 276–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Barker, T. A. (1994). The development of orthographic processing ability. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I:Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 243–276). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, C., & Willows, D. M. (1993). Evidence for a visual-processing-deficit subtype among disabled readers. In D. M. Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processes in reading and reading disabilities (pp. 287–309). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler intelligence scale for children-revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children-third edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willows, D. M., Kruk, R. S., & Corcos, E. (1993). Are there differences between disabled and normal readers in their processing of visual information? In D. M. Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processes in reading and reading disabilities (pp. 265–285). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willows, D. M., & Terepocki, M. (1993). The relation of reversal errors to reading disabilities. In D. M. Willows, R. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processes in reading and reading disabilities (pp. 31–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 123–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. (1997). A provisional, integrative account of phonological and naming-speed deficits in dyslexia: Implications for diagnosis and intervention. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Cognitive and linguistic foundations of reading acquisition: Implications for intervention research (pp. 67–92). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (2000). Naming-speed deficits in developmental reading disabilities: An introduction to the special series on the double-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 322–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Denckla, M. (2005). The rapid automatized naming and rapid alternating stimulus tests. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. H., & Melngailis, J. (1996). Reversing letters and reading transformed text in dyslexia: A reassignment. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 341–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock reading mastery tests-revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nathlie A. Badian.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Badian, N.A. Does a visual-orthographic deficit contribute to reading disability?. Ann. of Dyslexia 55, 28–52 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-005-0003-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-005-0003-x

Key Words

Navigation