Video games refer to any interactive games that are available to play on a number of different formats (e.g., arcade machines, gaming consoles, personal computers, tablets, smartphones, etc.) and played in a variety of modes (e.g., single player, online multiplayer, local co-op) and genres (e.g., action, adventure, strategy). Gaming has been shown to have positive effects in a wide range of areas (considering the individual’s general adaptation) including learning performance (Green and Bavelier 2012; Green et al. 2010), prosocial behavior (Greitemeyer and Osswald 2010; Gentile et al. 2009), mood regulation (Bowman and Tamborini 2012; Russoniello et al. 2009), and cognitive abilities (e.g., visual short term memory; Boot et al. 2008). However, excessive video gaming (both online and offline) has been associated with increased experiences of loneliness (Lemmens et al. 2011; Caplan et al. 2009), aggression (Kim et al. 2008; Caplan et al. 2009), depression (Caplan et al. 2009; Wei et al. 2012), social anxiety (Lee and Leeson 2015; Lo et al. 2005) and low self-esteem (Lemmens et al. 2011), as well as having positive applications in the health field (Griffiths et al. 2017). These associations may be explained using the Compensatory Internet Use hypothesis, which assumes that individuals who are less psychologically resilient are more likely to turn to online applications (i.e., video games) to compensate for their difficulties offline. For example, individuals who have fears regarding negative evaluation and difficulties with offline social settings may rely on the online gaming environment for more accessible social communication, as a less distressing environment (Valkenburg and Peter 2009, 2013). Therefore, online gaming can be a positive activity for some, and problematic for others, particularly vulnerable populations. With this understanding of individual factors as well as shared addictive processes (Griffiths 2005a), Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) was introduced as a potential disorder for further study in the latest (fifth) edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013).

IGD has been described as the relentless use of online videogames (generally with other players) that causes significant distress to the user (APA 2013). Similarities between IGD and other forms of addictions have been highlighted. More specifically, it has been suggested that IGD comprises common addiction components (Griffiths et al. 2014) such as mood modification (i.e., the way the object of addiction is used to modify the user’s mood), tolerance (i.e., the gradual building-up of resistance resulting in the need to engage more with the object of addiction), withdrawal symptoms (i.e., negative experiences when the object of the addiction is not present), conflict (compromising of all other things in the individual’s life such as educational/occupational duties, relationships, etc.), and relapse (i.e., reverting back to the addiction after a period of abstinence) (APA 2013; Anderson et al. 2016).

Several factors have been found to enhance IGD vulnerability. Indicatively, online games with a higher risk of developing IGD appear to share specific characteristics such as competing groups, teamwork, social communication, successive challenges, and accessibility to online games (APA 2013). Furthermore, comorbid psychopathological manifestations including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), major depressive disorder (MDD), and neglecting of one’s health have all been associated with IGD (APA 2013). Despite the significant progress in the field, further research considering risk and protective factors of IGD has been recommended (APA 2013). To further this aim, the present study explores the possible risk effect of anxiety and the possible protective effect of family cohesion experiences on the IGD risk of more anxious individuals.

Massively Multiplayer Online Games

Research has shown that not all players of online games are equally at-risk in developing IGD, which is due in part to the diverse range of game types available (Kuss and Griffiths 2012). One type, massively multiplayer online (MMO) games, are games in which players can interact with each other simultaneously, as they are connected via the online medium to a large player base, which offers opportunity for competitive and cooperative goals that can be both challenging and immersive (Steinkuehler and Williams 2006; Lee et al. 2015). In this context, MMOs require the development of an online character (avatar) through which the player interacts with both the virtual world and other players (Elliot et al. 2012). Research has consistently demonstrated excessive online use behaviors among MMO players and higher rates of IGD prevalence compared to players of other game genres (Caplan et al. 2009; Müller et al. 2014; Stavropoulos et al. 2016). More specifically, MMO players have been found to play longer, and exhibit irritability when not able to play (Berle et al. 2015), as well as lower self-regulation, increased impulsivity, and reduced agreeableness (Collins et al. 2012). These traits have been closely associated with other forms of addiction (Tayeebi et al. 2013), indicating that MMO players are a specific risk population. Based on this prior research, the present study focuses on MMO players during the significant (from a developmental perspective) and concurrently understudied period of emergent adulthood (Kuss and Griffiths 2012).

Emergent Adulthood

The age group of emergent adulthood is broadly defined as the 18–29 year age group in industrialized countries (Arnett 2000; Arnett et al. 2014). Arnett (2000) suggested that emergent adulthood is a time for change and discovery and is an important age range for scholarly attention. Simultaneously, emergent adulthood has been characterized as a critical period for the development of addictions more generally (Stone et al. 2012), and online-related addictions more particularly (Sussman and Arnett 2014). In Australia (where the present study was carried out), the vast majority of gamers are emergent adults, who typically play to relieve boredom and to have fun (Brand and Todhunter 2015). Interestingly, DSM-5 estimates the prevalence of IGD to be between 12 and 20% during the initiation phase of emergent adulthood (APA 2013)—although this was based on non-nationally representative survey studies—while recreational online use increases during the same period (Yen et al. 2009). Finally, many types of addictive behaviors that were initiated during emergent adulthood have been shown to continue over the life course (Ashenhurst, Harden, Corbin, & Fromme, 2015). Despite clear evidence to suggest the importance of studying IGD symptoms during emergent adulthood, much remains unknown about IGD in this age group, because the international literature has disproportionally emphasized adolescence (Müller et al. 2015; Rehbein et al. 2015).

Risk and Resilience Framework (RRF)

Taking into consideration previous empirical findings and contemporary research recommendations, the present study adopts the Risk and Resilience Framework (RRF) in examining IGD among emergent adult MMO gamers (Anderson et al. 2016; Arnett et al. 2014; Arnett 2000). Resilience is defined as the combination of aspects and experience (i.e., protective factors and resources) that allow an individual to be unaffected by something problematic (Masten 2001). In contrast, risk is defined as involving aspects and experiences of individuals that make them susceptible to developing problematic disorders and behaviors (Grewirtz and Edleson 2007). In this context, the RRF posits that behaviors constantly vary on a continuum because of the interplay between developmental (age-related), individual (person-related), and contextual risk and protective factors (Masten 2014). In the present study, risk and RRF are used to explore IGD risk factors at the individual level (e.g., anxiety) and IGD resilience factors at the contextual level (e.g., family cohesion).

Anxiety

A significant relationship between anxiety and addictive behaviors, and technological addictions in particular, has been repeatedly demonstrated (Andreassen et al. 2016; Buckner and Schmidt 2009; Cooper et al. 2014; Essau et al. 2014; Lee and Stapinski 2012; Parhami et al. 2014; Sareen et al. 2006; Van Der Maas 2016). Furthermore, anxiety-related factors such as sensitivity to stress, perceived uncontrollability, and avoidance have also been shown to be associated with addiction severity (Forsyth et al. 2003). Nevertheless, the association between anxiety and addiction is considered bidirectional (Kushner et al. 2000). Anxiety and addictive behaviors can both initiate the other, and anxiety can contribute to the maintenance of, and relapse to an addictive behavior (Dalbudak et al. 2014). This has broadly been explained on the basis of addictive behaviors (such as IGD) providing immediate gratification and relief from anxiety, and thus functioning as maladaptive anxiety addressing mechanisms (Douglas et al. 2008; Griffiths 2005a, b; Griffiths et al. 2016; Marlatt and Donovan 2005; Mehroof and Griffiths 2010). Along this line, Caplan (2002) suggests that social anxiety predicts preference for online social interactions and leads to negative outcomes associated with problematic Internet use. This is in consensus with several studies suggesting an association between anxiety manifestations and excessive Internet use (Cole and Hooley 2013; Lee and Leeson 2015; Lo et al. 2005). Despite these few studies, there is generally a dearth of specialized (longitudinal) research emphasizing on potential buffers (i.e., protective factors) of anxiety’s effect on IGD. The present study therefore contributes to the field by exploring the potential protective role of family cohesion in both a cross-sectional and longitudinal manner.

Family Cohesion

In addition to anxiety, contextual factors have been implicated as being critical for the development of behavior in general (Cicchetti and Toth 2009), addictions in general (Griffiths 2005a, b), and technological addictions in particular (Douglas et al. 2008). More specifically, IGD-related studies have highlighted the need to pay equal attention to individual, cultural and online contexts (Kuss 2013). In that line, family contextual factors including parenting attitudes, family communication, and cohesion have been found to act protectively for excessive Internet use, whereas others, such as exposure to family conflicts, have been shown to increase it (Park et al. 2008). In this context, the significance of family cohesion has been illustrated for understanding addictive behaviors (Reeb et al. 2015). Family cohesion is described as the “emotional bonding among family members and the feeling of closeness that is expressed by feelings of belonging and acceptance within the family” (Johnson et al. 2001, p. 306). Cohesive families appear to function in a number of ways that may reduce addictive behaviors (i.e., binge drinking and smoking initiation; Rajesh et al. 2015; Solloski et al. 2015). In particular, cohesive families appear likely to decrease IGD escaping motives (i.e., individuals may not abscond to the virtual world due to their satisfying family context). Furthermore, such families may positively moderate other external stressors (i.e., peer issues), that could precipitate IGD behaviors, by providing a place of belonging, and a safe outlet for relieving tension (Rajesh et al. 2015; Solloski et al. 2015). This is in consensus with literature indicating family cohesion to have a buffering role in the associations between discomfort and distress and psychopathological behaviors, such as post-traumatic stress disorder (Farrell et al. 1995; Ibanez et al. 2015; Kaur and Kearney 2013). Subsequently, one might assume that higher levels of family cohesion moderate (i.e., buffer) IGD risk for more anxious individuals. The rationale for this hypothesis is twofold. Firstly, adaptive emotion regulation strategies through communication and sharing are promoted in more cohesive families (Li and Warner 2015) and therefore, individuals may have less urge to abscond online. Secondly, family cohesion has been related to greater connectedness and, as expected, is inversely related to neglect (Manzi et al. 2006; Wark et al. 2003). It therefore follows that a strong family base could reduce IGD risk. The present study is (to the best of the authors’ knowledge) the first that aims to assess this hypothesis.

The Present Study

The present study utilizes the RRF to explore the IGD risk effect of anxiety (as a factor related to the gamer) and the potential buffering effect of family cohesion on this association (as a moderating factor related to the gamer’s context). More specifically, a combination of cross-sectional and longitudinal data of Australian MMO gamers, aged 18–29 years is examined to address the following two hypotheses:

  • H1: Based on the available literature, it was hypothesized that higher anxiety symptoms would function (both cross-sectionally and longitudinally) as an IGD risk factor (i.e., individuals with higher anxiety levels will report higher IGD scores; Ahmadi et al., 2014; Dalbudak et al. 2014).

  • H2: It was hypothesized that higher levels of family cohesion would buffer (i.e., negatively moderate) the effect of anxiety on IGD symptoms over time (Kaur & Kearney 2013; Ibanez et al. 2015; Johnson et al. 2001; Priest & Denton, 2012).

Method

Participants

Participants involved in the cross-sectional (online and face-to-face) component of the present study were 125 emerging adultsFootnote 1 (Mage = 23.34, SD = 3.29, Minage = 18, Maxage = 29, Males = 49, 77.6%). Additionally, 61 participants were involved in the longitudinal (face-to-face) component (Mage = 23.34, SD = 3.39, Minage = 18, Maxage = 29, Males = 49, 77.6%). To ensure there were no significant differences between the cross-sectional and longitudinal participants on each of the variables, independent samples t tests and chi-squared analyses were conducted. No significant differences were found.Footnote 2 Time point 1 (T1), Time point 2 (T2) and Time Point 3 (T3) (longitudinal data) were used to assess the longitudinal part of Hypothesis 1 and Hypothesis 2 related to over time effects.Footnote 3 The estimated maximum sampling errors with a size of 125 (cross-sectional sample) and a size of 61 (longitudinal sample) are 8.77 and 12.55% (Z = 1.96, confidence level 95%) respectively. Demographic characteristics of the sample are listed in Table 1. For sample collection process see Fig. 1.

Table 1 Participants’ demographic characteristics
Fig. 1
figure 1

Sample collection process

Measures

Internet Gaming Disorder Scale–Short Form 9 (IGDS-SF9)

Pontes and Griffiths’ (2015) IGS-SF9 includes nine items that assess the degree of Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) symptoms. The IGDS-SF9 is based on the nine criteria for IGD outlined in the DSM-5 (APA 2013). Participants were asked to respond on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (Never) to 5 (Very often) concerning their gaming behaviors (e.g., “Do you feel more irritability, anxiety or even sadness when you try to either reduce or stop your gaming activity?”). The score derived is a composite score, 9 (minimal IGD symptoms) to 45 (maximum IGD symptoms). The instrument demonstrated high Internal reliability (Cronbach α = .92) in the present study.

Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI)

The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) is a 21-item scale used to assess participants’ anxiety (Beck and Steer 1990). Participants were asked to indicate how much a symptom “bothered” them in the past month (e.g., “Feeling hot”), ranging from 1 (Not at all) to 4 (Severely). Total scores were generated by summing all items, resulting in a range from 21(lower severity) to 84 (higher severity). Higher and lower scores indicate higher and lower severity of anxiety symptoms respectively. In the present study, internal consistency was excellent (Cronbach α = .95).

Balanced Family Cohesion Subscale

To assess family cohesion, and in consensus with previous studies (Jin 2015), the Balanced Family Cohesion Subscale of the Family Adaptability and Cohesion Evaluation Scale-FACES IV (Olson 2011) was used. The scale comprised of seven items, with participants required to answer questions that refer to the balance (e.g., connected without being emotionally disengaged and enmeshed) connectedness with their family members (e.g., “Although family members have individual interests, they still participate in family activities”). Participants were required to respond on a scale ranging from 1 (Never or definitely) to 5 (Strongly agree), items’ scores were added resulting to a range of 7 to 35. Higher scores indicate higher levels of balanced family cohesion and lower scores indicate lower levels of balanced family cohesion. In the present study the scale’s internal consistency was considered good to excellent (Cronbach α = .89).

Procedure

The project was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the authors’ university. Participants were recruited from the general community (including staff and students of the university) using traditional (i.e., information flyers) and electronic (i.e., email, social media) advertising methods.Footnote 4 The longitudinal sample was collected face-to-face over a three-month period between June 2016 and September 2016. Mutually agreed data collection appointments (25–35 min) were arranged with participants by a trained research team of five undergraduate students and two postgraduate students. No compensation was provided to the participants.

Online collection took place over a 1-month period (June 1, 2016 and July 1, 2016). Eligible individuals interested in participating and unable or unwilling to attend face-to-face testing sessions were invited to register with the study via a Survey-Monkey link available on MMO websites and forums. Individuals that chose to participate online were provided with a brief description of the study (i.e., methods, aims, conditions) and directed to click a button to provide informed consent (i.e., digitally sign).

Statistical Analysis

To assess H1, two linear regression analyses (one cross-sectional and one longitudinal) were conducted to determine if anxiety was an IGD risk factor. To assess H2, a moderation analysis was conducted via Process software (Hayes 2013) to determine if the interaction between anxiety and balanced family cohesion positively moderated (buffered) IGD symptoms. The longitudinal data were used to explore this relationship following relevant literature recommendations, which suggest longitudinal data is required to support causative associations (Winer et al. 2016). In accordance with these suggestions, participants’ anxiety scores at T1 were entered as the independent variable, balanced family cohesion at T2 was entered as the moderator, and IGD at T3 was used as the outcome variable. Finally, the Johnson Neyman (J–N) technique was applied to derive regions of significance (i.e., points of transition) of the moderation effect of balanced family cohesion on the association between anxiety and IGD symptoms (Preacher et al. 2007).

Results

The analyses’ assumptions, descriptive statistics and inter-correlations between the examined variables were assessed before calculating the results. To dimensionally assess (on a continuum from minimum to maximum) the association between anxiety and IGD symptoms H1, a linear regression analysis was first conducted on the cross-sectional data. More specifically, IGD symptoms were inserted as the dependent variable and anxiety was inserted as an independent variable. The effect of anxiety accounted for a significant 23.6% of the IGD variance (R2 = .24, F(1, 120) = 36.98, p < .001). According to the regression coefficient, one-unit increase in anxiety predicted a .32 increase in IGD scores (b = .32, SE(b) = .05, β  = .49, p < .001).

The linear regression model described above was repeated using the longitudinal data. IGD symptoms assessed at T3 were inserted as the dependent variable, and anxiety at T1 as the independent variable. Anxiety at T1 accounted for a significant 8.9% of the variance in IGD symptoms at T3, R2 = .09, F(1, 56) = 5.48, p = .023. More specifically, one-unit of increase in anxiety at T1 predicted .16 of increase in IGD symptoms at T3 (b = .16, SE (b) = .07, β = 30, t = 2.34, p < .05).

To address H2, a moderation analysis was conducted adopting the methodology recommended by Hayes (2013). The model examined the moderating (buffering) effect of balanced family cohesion (Moderator) at T2 on the association between anxiety (IV) at T1 and IGD (DV) symptoms at T3. The model estimated the following:

IGD T3 = a + banxiety T1 + bbalanced family cohesion T2 + b3 [anxiety T1 × balanced family cohesion T2];

Findings indicated that the full model accounted for 17.88% of variance in IGD at T3. The slope of the regression line for the overall model was significant (F(3, 54) = 3.92, p = .01). An inspection of the interaction coefficient indicated that anxiety at T1 and balanced family cohesion at T2 significantly interacted (protective effect) in predicting IGD scores at T3, b = − .04, t(54) = −2.30, p = .03. Findings indicated that when an individual presented concurrently with a higher point (than the intercept) in anxiety at T1 and a higher point in balanced family cohesion at T2, then IGD symptoms at T3 reduce by .04. A visual representation of this buffering/protective effect is illustrated in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Moderating effect of balanced family cohesion (T2) on the relationship between Internet Gambling Disorder and anxiety

Finally, the Johnson Neyman (J–N) technique was applied to derive regions of significance (points of transition) of the moderation effect in the sample (Preacher et al. 2007). Findings demonstrated that the buffering effect of balanced family cohesion at T2 on the association between anxiety at T1 and IGD at T3, started from the minimum values of the moderator and had a ceiling point for values of balanced family cohesion exceeding 29.74. This means that after this point, the moderation/buffering effect of balanced family cohesion at T2 on the association between anxiety at T1 and IGD at T3 had reached its maximum protective effect, b = .06, t(54) = .66, p = .51, (see Fig. 2).

Discussion

The present study used a Risk and Resilience Framework (RRF) approach to examine cross-sectional and over time variations (three points across a period of 3 months, 1 month apart each) of IGD behaviors among an Australian sample of MMO gamers, assessed online and/or face-to-face. More specifically, it investigated whether higher anxiety levels functioned as an IGD risk factor; and whether higher levels of family cohesion reduced IGD risk of more anxious individuals. Linear regression and moderation analyses demonstrated that higher anxiety associated with increased IGD behaviors both cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Furthermore, higher anxiety gamers were significantly less susceptible to IGD over time, when they experienced higher levels of family cohesion. These findings imply the need to consider the interplay between individual- and family-related factors when examining IGD and planning relevant prevention and intervention strategies.

Anxiety

Using the RRF (Masten 2014), anxiety was shown to be a factor (related to the player) that increased IGD risk. More specifically, results suggested that emergent adult MMO players who reported higher anxiety were inclined to have higher IGD scores. This is in consensus with previous research indicating the role of anxiety as a risk factor for addictions in general (Buckner and Schmidt 2009; Cooper et al. 2014; Parhami et al. 2014; Sareen et al. 2006; Van Der Maas 2016), and for online-related addictive behaviors in particular (Andreassen et al. 2016; Lee and Stapinski 2012).

It is likely that excessive use of online games may relieve symptoms of anxiety (Mehroof and Griffiths 2010). This explanation is in accord with the Compensatory Internet Use hypothesis, which suggests that individuals who are less psychologically resilient may turn to online communications (e.g., via online games) to compensate for their difficulties offline (i.e. anxiety; Valkenburg and Peter 2009, 2013). Applying this to the present study, it would mean that participants with higher anxiety levels are more likely turn to online games to compensate for their real-world life difficulties, where they are more anxious, because this would provide relief from the thoughts or experiences that may precipitate and/or perpetuate their anxiety symptoms. Conceptually, there are several aspects of a person’s real life such as social interaction and fear of negative evaluation, which could act as anxiety triggers (i.e., stress-related issues) and appear not to be evident whilst gaming (Mehroof & Griffiths 2010). In this context, the Compensatory Internet Use Hypothesis for explaining the IGD risk of more anxious gamers aligns with an extensive body of literature, which suggests that addictive behaviors (in general) provide immediate gratification and relief from anxiety and distress (Akin and İskender 2011; Douglas et al. 2008; Griffiths 2005a, b; Griffiths et al. 2016).

Family Cohesion as a Buffer of the IGD Risk Effect of Anxiety

Interestingly, the present study demonstrated that more anxious emergent adults, who play MMOs and experienced higher levels of family cohesion were at lower IGD risk (than their equally anxious co-players, who reported lower family cohesion levels). Additionally, a ceiling effect was found for high scores of family cohesion, meaning that after a specific point, higher levels of family cohesion would not further reduce the IGD risk of more anxious gamers. Along this line, the buffering effect that family cohesion had on the association between anxiety and IGD was substantially stronger when family cohesion increased between low and average levels. To the best of the author’s knowledge, this is the first study that has examined the potential protective effect of higher family cohesion for more anxious gamers. Nevertheless, the findings were in consensus with previous research suggesting that family cohesion may have a significant role in preventing the development of addictions (Solloski et al. 2015; Rajesh et al. 2015; Manzi et al. 2006; Wark et al. 2003; Li and Warner 2015). There are two potential reasons why this might be the case. Firstly, adaptive emotion regulation strategies via communication and sharing are promoted in more cohesive families (Li and Warner 2015), and therefore, individuals may have less urge to abscond online (via excessive gaming). Secondly, family cohesion has been related to increased connectedness and less neglect (Manzi et al. 2006; Wark et al. 2003). This may mean that individual players would be more likely to talk and gain relief regarding issues causing them anxiety within more cohesive families. These options could deter more anxious gamers from the excessive use of online games to address their anxiety-related discomfort and distress.

However, reinforcing family cohesion is not a panacea for IGD in more anxious gamers. The ceiling effect indicated that higher family cohesion positively moderates the IGD risk until a specific point and can be more effective when raising the family cohesion levels from low to average. This could be interpreted as the result of family cohesion levels not exclusively moderating the IGD risk of more anxious gamers (Anderson et al. 2016). Other factors, such as game-related features (i.e., avatar graphs, gaming structure) may also contribute and/or accommodate gaming over-involvement of more anxious individuals, and their effect could be more significant when family cohesion is experienced (Anderson et al. 2016). Future research should build upon these findings by more clearly delineating the potential basis of family cohesion’s positive role and providing guidelines for more effective IGD prevention and treatment efforts.

Limitations, Implications, Future Research and Conclusions

The present study has several strengths including (i) a concurrent examination of online and face-to-face participants; (ii) a combination of cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses; (iii) an emphasis on an under-researched high risk population for IGD (emergent adult MMO gamers), and (iv) a focus on both individual- and contextual-related factors, as well as their interplay in relation to IGD.

However, there are several limitations that need to be considered. Firstly, IGD was assessed by focusing on a specific group of online gaming genres. There are multiple online gaming genres, and therefore characteristics of those who play them may vary accordingly. Secondly, the sample size was relatively small (N = 125), suggesting that limitations may exist when extrapolating the results. Thirdly, the longitudinal design applied referred to a relatively short timeframe. Fourth, the present study used self-report measures that may have affected reliability because participant responses can be influenced by mood and situational factors at the time when the assessment occurred (Smith & Handler, 2007). Finally, different directions of the causal relationships between the variables involved in the study could have been examined (i.e., low family cohesion mediated by anxiety on IGD risk).

Within the context of the aforementioned limitations, the findings have several important clinical implications. Considering IGD prevention, the findings suggest that individuals who report higher anxiety need to be prioritized, especially when they are situated in families with lower family cohesion levels. In relation to treatment directions, cognitive-behavior therapy may be useful to promote awareness in regard to the use of IGD as a potential maladaptive emotional compensation strategy. Furthermore, family therapy promoting higher family cohesion levels may prove beneficial in reducing IGD risk of higher anxiety players.

In line with these suggestions, the present study prompts several further research avenues within the IGD field. Future research should more deeply investigate the longitudinal interplay between individual (i.e. age, gender, societal, cultural background) and gaming-related factors (i.e. game genre, game structure, character development). Furthermore, increasing the length of the longitudinal design could greatly benefit knowledge considering time-related variations in the association between anxiety and IGD, and well as the anxiety-family cohesion interaction. Investigating other potential protective factors that reduce the association between anxiety and IGD may also add to the extant knowledge.