Skip to main content
Log in

Dynamic reasoning and time pressure: Transition from analytical operations to experiential responses

  • Published:
Theory and Decision Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Based upon the Decision Field Theory (Busemeyer and Townsend 1993), we tested a model of dynamic reasoning to predict the effect of time pressure on analytical and experiential processing during decision-making. Forty-six participants were required to make investment decisions under four levels of time pressure. In each decision, participants were presented with experiential cues which were either congruent or incongruent with the analytical information. The congruent/incongruent conditions allowed us to examine how many decisions were based upon the experiential versus the analytical information, and to see if this was affected by the varying degrees of time pressure. As expected, the overall accuracy was reduced with greater time pressure and accuracy was higher when the experiential and analytical cues were congruent than when they were incongruent. Of great interest was the data showing that under high time pressure participants used more experiential cues than at other time pressures. We suggest that the dynamic reasoning paradigm has some future potential for predicting the effects of experiential biases in general, and specifically under time pressure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Abbreviations

DFT:

Decision field theory

RAT:

Rational model

TTB:

Take-the-best model

EXP:

Experiential system output

References

  • Bechara A., Damasio H., Tranel D., Damasio A. R. (1997) Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275: 1293–1295

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Zur H., Breznitz S. J. (1981) The effects of time pressure on risky choice behaviour. Acta Psychologica 81: 211–241

    Google Scholar 

  • Berthoz A. (2006) Emotion and reason: The cognitive neuroscience of decision-making. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio A. R. (1994) Descartes’ error: Emotion reason, and the human brain. Putnam, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Dror I. E., Busemeyer J. R., Basola B. (1999) Decision making under time pressure: An independent test of sequential sampling models. Memory and Cognition 27(4): 713–725

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans J. St. B. T., Barston J. L., Pollard P. (1983) On the conflict between logic and belief in syllogistic reasoning. Memory and Cognition 11: 295–306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finucane M.L., Peters E., Slovic P. (2003) Judgement and decision-making: The dance of affect and reason. In: Schneider S.L., Shanteau J. (eds) Emerging perspectives on judgement and decision research. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 327–364

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman W., Watts J. W. (1942) Psychosurgery intelligence emotion and social behavior following prefrontal lobotomy for mental disorders. Charles C Thomas, Springfield, IL

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Freund T., Kruglanski A. W., Shpitzajzen A. (1985) The freezing and unfreezing of impressional primacy: Effects of the need for structure and the fear of invalidity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 11: 479–487

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer G., Todd P. M. (1999) Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzales C., Dana J., Koshino H., Just M. (2005) The framing effect and risky decisions: Examining cognitive functions with fMRI. Journal of Economic Psychology 26: 1–20

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogarth R. M. (2001) Educating intuition. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D., Frederick S. (2002) Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgement. In: Gilovich Griffin D., Kahneman (eds) Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgement. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 49–81

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski A. W., Freund T. (1983) The freezing and unfreezing of lay-inferences: Effects on impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping, and numerical anchoring. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 19: 448–468

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunda Z. (1990) The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin 108: 480–498

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee M. D., Cummins T. D. R. (2004) Evidence accumulation in decision making: Unifying the “take the best” and the “rational” models. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 11(2): 343–352

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein G. F., Weber E. U., Hsee C. K., Welch E. S. (2001) Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin 127: 267–286

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne J. W., Bettman J. R. (2004) The information-processing approach to decision making. In: Koehler D. J., Harvey N. (eds) Blackwell Handbook of Judgement and Decision Making.. Blackwell Publishing, London, pp 110–132

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Payne J.W., Bettman J.R., Johnson E.J. (1993) The adaptive decision maker. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls E. T. (1999) The brain and emotion. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman S. A. (1996) The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin 119: 3–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Svenson O., Edland A. (1987) Change in preferences under time pressure: Choices and judgements. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 29(4): 322–330

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallsten T. S. (1993) The pressure and payoff effects on multidimensional probabilistic inference. In: Svenson O., Maule A. J. (eds) Time pressure and stress in human judgement and decision making.. Plenum, New York, pp 167–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson T. D., Schooler J. W. (1991) Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60(2): 181–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter A. F. Fraser-Mackenzie.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fraser-Mackenzie, P.A.F., Dror, I.E. Dynamic reasoning and time pressure: Transition from analytical operations to experiential responses. Theory Decis 71, 211–225 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11238-009-9181-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11238-009-9181-z

Keywords

Navigation