Skip to main content
Log in

Longitudinal changes in college math students’ implicit theories of intelligence

  • Published:
Social Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined changes over time in implicit theories of intelligence and their relationships to help-seeking and academic performance. College algebra students completed questionnaires during the second week of classes and 2 weeks before the end of the semester (ns = 159 and 145, respectively; 61 students completed questionnaires at both waves). The questionnaires assessed entity and incremental implicit theories of general and math intelligence (beginning and end of semester) and help-seeking (end of semester). Results indicated that students had more incremental views of general than math intelligence. Further, their views became less incremental over the course of the semester; however, this decrease was greater for math than for general intelligence. Participants who exhibited a stronger incremental theory of general intelligence at the beginning of the semester subsequently reported greater help-seeking during the semester. Finally, students who had more entitative views of math intelligence earned lower course grades.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ahmavaara A., Houston D. M. (2007) The effects of selective schooling and self-concept on adolescents’ academic aspiration: An examination of Dweck’s self-theory. British Journal of Educational Psychology 77: 613–632. doi:10.1348/000709906X120132

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Augustine, N. R. (2005). Rising above the gathering storm: Energizing and employing America for a brighter economic future. Retrieved from http://sciencedems.house.gov/Media/File/Reports/natacad_compete_exsum_6feb06.pdf.

  • Baumeister R. F., Campbell J. D., Krueger J. L., Vohs K. D. (2003) Does high self-esteem cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness or healthier lifestyles?. Psychological Science in the Public Interest 4: 1–44. doi:10.1111/1529-1006.01431

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell L. S., Trzesniewski K., Dweck C. S. (2007) Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development 78: 246–263. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00995.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burns K. C., Isbell L. M. (2007) Promoting malleability is not one size fits all: Priming implicit theories of intelligence as a function of self-theories. Self and Identity 6: 51–63. doi:10.1080/15298860600823864

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ceci, S. J., Williams, W. M. (Eds.) (2007) Why aren’t there more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiu C., Dweck C. S., Tong J. Y., Fu J. H. (1997) Implicit theories and conceptions of morality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73: 923–940. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.73.5.923

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu C., Hong Y., Dweck C. S. (1997) Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73: 19–30. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.73.1.19

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dutton K. A., Brown J. D. (1997) Global self-esteem and specific self-views as determinants of people’s reactions to success and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73: 139–148. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.73.1.139

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck C. S. (1999) Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Psychology Press, Philadelphia, PA

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck C. S. (2006) Is math a gift? Beliefs that put females at risk. In: Ceci S. J., Williams W. M. (Eds.) Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck C. S., Hong Y., Chiu C. (1993) Implicit theories: Individual differences in the likelihood and meaning of dispositional inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 19: 644–656. doi:10.1177/0146167293195015

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck C. S., Leggett E. L. (1988) A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review 95: 256–273. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.95.2.256

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck C. S., & Molden D. C. (2005). Self-theories: Their impact on competence motivation and acquisition. In: Elliot A., & Dweck C. S. Handbook of competence and motivation. (pp 122–140) New York: Guilford Press.

  • Erdley C., Loomis C., Cain K., Dumas-Hines F., Dweck C. S. (1997) The relations among children’s social goals, implicit personality theories and response to social failure. Developmental Psychology 33: 263–272. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.33.2.263

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grant H., Dweck C. S. (2003) Clarifying achievement goals and their impact. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85: 541–553. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.541

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill C., Corbett C., St. Rose A. (2010) Why so few? Women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics. AAUW, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Hong Y. Y., Chiu C., Dweck C. S. (1995) Implicit theories of intelligence: Reconsidering the role of confidence in achievement motivation. In: Kernis M. (Ed.) Efficacy, agency, and self-esteem. Plenum, New York, pp 197–216

    Google Scholar 

  • Hong Y., Chiu C., Dweck C. S., Lin D. M. S., Wan W. (1999) Implicit theories, attributions, and coping: A meaning system approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77: 588–599. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.77.3.588

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • H. R. 2272–110th Congress: America COMPETES Act. (2007). In GovTrack.us (database of federal legislation). Retrieved from http://www.govtrack.us/congress/bills/110/hr2272.

  • H. R. 5116–111th Congress: America COMPETES Reauthorization Act of 2010. (2010). In GovTrack.us (database of federal legislation). Retrieved from http://www.govtrack.us/congress/bills/111/hr5116.

  • Hyde J. S., Lindberg S. M., Linn M. C., Ellis A. B., Williams C. C. (2008) Gender similarities characterize math performance. Science 312: 494–495. doi:10.1126/science.1160364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, R. B., McInerney, D. M., & Watkins, D. A. (2012). How you think about your intelligence determines how you feel in school: The role of theories of intelligence on academic emotions. Learning and Individual Differences. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.04.005.

  • Midgley C., Arunkumar R., Urdan T. C. (1996) “If I don’t do well tomorrow, there’s a reason:” Predictors of adolescents’ use of academic self-handicapping strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology 88: 423–434. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.88.3.423

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller C. H., Burgoon J. K., Hall J. R. (2007) The effects of implicit theories of moral character on affective reactions to moral transgressions. Social Cognition 25: 819–832. doi:10.1521/soco.2007.25.6.819

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaks J. E., Dweck C. S., Grant H. (2005) Violations of implicit theories and the sense of prediction and control: Implications for motivated person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88: 245–262. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.667

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rattan, A., Savani, K., Naidu, N. V. R., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). Can everyone become highly intelligent? Cultural differences in and societal consequences of beliefs about the universal potential for intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. doi:10.1037/a0029263.

  • Renaud J., McConnell A. (2007) Wanting to be better but thinking you can’t: Implicit theories of personality moderate the impact of self-discrepancies on self-esteem. Self and Identity 6: 41–50. doi:10.1080/15298860600764597

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodewalt F. (1994) Conceptions of ability, achievement goals, and individual differences in self-handicapping behavior: On the application of implicit theories. Journal of Personality 62: 67–85. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00795.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robins R. W., Pals J. L. (2002) Implicit self-theories in the academic domain: Implications for goal orientation, attributions, affect, and self-esteem change. Self and Identity 1: 313–336. doi:10.1080/15298860290106805

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour E., Hewitt N. M. (1997) Talking about leaving: Why undergraduates leave the sciences. Westview Press, Boulder, CO

    Google Scholar 

  • Shih S. (2011) Perfectionism, implicit theories of intelligence, and Taiwanese eight-grade students’ academic engagement. The Journal of Educational Research 104: 131–142. doi:10.1080/00220670903570368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valian V. (2006) Women at the top in science-and elsewhere. In: Ceci S.J., Williams W. M. (Eds.) Why aren’t more women in science? Top researchers debate the evidence. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca L. Shively.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Shively, R.L., Ryan, C.S. Longitudinal changes in college math students’ implicit theories of intelligence. Soc Psychol Educ 16, 241–256 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-012-9208-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-012-9208-0

Keywords

Navigation