Skip to main content
Log in

The Cognitive Construction of Gender Stereotypes: Evidence for the Dual Pathways Model of Gender Differentiation

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The dual pathways model of gender differentiation suggests two possible pathways that examine the relationships between gender-typed interests and gender stereotypes: (a) an attitudinal pathway model, which suggests that stereotypes may shape interests for gender-schematic children, and (b) a personal pathway model, which suggests that personal interests may lead to the construction of stereotypes that are in line with one’s interests (Liben and Bigler 2002). In Study 1, the personal pathway model was tested by presenting children in the United States (N = 51, M age  = 3.99 years-old) with novel toys and assessing the congruence between interests and stereotypes. Results indicated a significant relationship between personal interests and stereotypes such that higher levels of personal interest in toys were associated with more congruent gender stereotypes—stereotypes in which the children endorsed the belief that the toy was only for same-gender children or for both genders. In Study 2, the attitudinal pathway was tested by presenting children in the United States (N = 57, M age  = 4.03 years-old) with novel toys that were labeled as “for boys” or “for girls.” Results indicated that gender-schematic children (those who endorsed higher levels of cultural gender stereotypes), but not gender-aschematic children (those who endorsed lower levels of cultural gender stereotypes) were more interested in toys labeled as for their gender than toys labeled as for the other gender. Thus, the results demonstrate that the pathways between these two constructs may be dependent on environmental information, individual differences in personal interest, and gender schematicity as predicted by the dual pathways model.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88(4), 354–364. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.88.4.354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1983). Gender schema theory and its implications for child development: Raising gender-aschematic children in a gender-schematic society. Signs, 8(4), 598–616. doi:10.1086/493998.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 162–166. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00496.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R. S., & Signorella, M. L. (2011). Single-sex education: New perspectives and evidence on a continuing controversy. Sex Roles, 65, 659–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, J., Berenbaum, S. A., & Liben, L. S. (2009). Gender development. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradbard, M. R., Martin, C. L., Endsley, R. C., & Halverson, C. F. (1986). Influence of sex stereotypes on children’s exploration and memory: A competence versus performance distinction. Developmental Psychology, 22(4), 481–486. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.22.4.481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. B. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, D. B., & Levy, G. D. (1988). Cognitive aspects of early sex role development: The influence of gender schemas on preschoolers’ memories and preferences for sex-typed toys and activities. Child Development, 59, 782–792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coyle, E., & Liben, L. S. (2016). Affecting girls’ activity and job interests through play: The moderating roles of personal gender salience and game characteristics. Child Development, 87, 414–428.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frabel, D. E. S., & Bem, S. L. (1985). If you are gender schematic, all members of the opposite sex look alike. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 459–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fulcher, M. (2011). Individual differences in children’s occupational aspirations as a function of parent traditionality. Sex Roles, 64, 117–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, D., Eliot, L., Bigler, R. S., Fabes, R. A., Hanish, L. D., Hyde, J., … Martin, C. L. (2011, September 23). The pseudoscience of single-sex schooling. Science, 333, 1706–1707.

  • Hilliard, L. J., & Liben, L. S. (2010). Differing levels of gender salience in preschool classrooms: Effects on children’s gender attitudes and intergroup bias. Child Development, 81, 1787–1798.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children’s sex-role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 82–173). Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lam, V. L., & Leman, P. J. (2003). The influence of gender and ethnicity on children’s inferences about toy choice. Social Development, 12(2), 269–287. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liben, L. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2002). The developmental course of gender differentiation: Conceptualizing, measuring, and evaluating constructs and pathways. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(2), 1–147. doi:10.1111/1540-5834.t01-1-00187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsey, A. E., & Zakahi, W. R. (1996). Women who tell and men who ask: Perceptions of men and women departing from gender stereotypes during initial interaction. Sex Roles, 34, 767–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lobel, T. E., & Menashri, J. (1993). Relations of conceptions of gender-role transgressions and gender constancy to gender-typed toy preferences. Developmental Psychology, 29, 150–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. (2000). Cognitive theories of gender development. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 91–121). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Dinella, L. M. (2012). Congruence between gender stereotypes and activity preference in self-identified tomboys and non-tomboys. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41(3), 599–610. doi:10.1007/s10508-011-9786-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52(4), 1119–1134. doi:10.2307/1129498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues: Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 67–70. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00276.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H. (1995). Children’s gender-based reasoning about toys. Child Development, 66, 1453–1471. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00945.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, C., Martin, C. L., Fabes, R., & Hanish, L. D. (2013). Bringing the cognitive and social together: How gender detectives and gender enforcers shape children’s gender development (pp. 306-313). In M. Banaji & S. Gelman (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 306–313). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M., Peyton, V., Mistry, R., Hruda, L., Jacobs, A., Caldera, Y., … Roy, C. (2000). Gender-role cognition in three-year-old boys and girls. Sex Roles, 42, 1007–1025.

  • Orenstein, P. (2011). Cinderella ate my daughter: Dispatches from the front lines of the new girly-girl culture. New York: HarperCollins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, M. M. (2012). Self-perceived gender typicality, gender-typed attributes, and gender stereotype endorsement in elementary-school-aged children. Sex Roles, 67(7-8), 422–434. doi:10.1007/s11199-012-0184-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prentice, D. A. (2002). What women should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 269–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., Taylor, L. J., Cyphers, L., Greulich, F. K., Lurye, L. E., & Shrout, P. E. (2007). The role of gender constancy in early gender development. Child Development, 78(4), 1121–1136. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01056.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Signorella, M. L., & Frieze, I. (2008). Interrelations of gender schemas in children and adolescents: Attitudes, preferences, and self-perceptions. Social Behavior and Personality, 36(7), 941–954. doi:10.2224/sbp.2008.36.7.941.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Signorella, M. L., Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1993). Developmental differences in children’s gender schemata about others: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 13(2), 147–183. doi:10.1006/drev.1993.1007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sjöberg, O. (2004). The role of family policy institutions in explaining gender-role attitudes: A comparative multilevel analysis of thirteen industrialized countries. Journal of European Social Policy, 14, 107–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweet, E. V. (2013, August). Same as it ever was? Gender and children’s toys over the 20th century. Presented at the 108th Annual American Sociological Association meeting, New York, NY.

  • Trautner, H. M., Ruble, D. N., Cyphers, L., Kirsten, B., Behrendt, R., & Hartmann, P. (2005). Rigidity and flexibility of gender stereotypes in childhood: Developmental or differential? Infant and Child Development, 14(4), 365–381. doi:10.1002/icd.399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weisgram, E. S., Fulcher, M., & Dinella, L. S. (2014). Pink gives girls permission: Exploring the roles of explicit gender labels and gender-typed colors on preschool children’s toy preferences. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35, 401–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilansky-Traynor, P., & Lobel, T. E. (2008). Differential effects of an adult observer’s presence on sex-typed play behavior: A comparison between gender-schematic and gender-aschematic preschool children. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37(4), 548–557. doi:10.1007/s10508-008-9342-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, E., Desmarais, S., & Gugula, S. (2002). The impact of parenting experience on gender stereotyped toy play of children. Sex Roles, 47, 39–49. doi:10.1023/A:1020679619728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The author would like to extend great appreciation to Carol Martin and Lisa Dinella who provided comments on a previous draft. In addition, the author would like to thank Erin Watson, Charis Zmudzinski, Jacylyn Wahl, James LeClair, Cathy Morrow, and Debra Simmerman for their assistance in collecting and entering the data and for their bibliographic assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erica S. Weisgram.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Weisgram, E.S. The Cognitive Construction of Gender Stereotypes: Evidence for the Dual Pathways Model of Gender Differentiation. Sex Roles 75, 301–313 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0624-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0624-z

Keywords

Navigation