Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Women in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM): An Investigation of Their Implicit Gender Stereotypes and Stereotypes’ Connectedness to Math Performance

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In spite of many barriers facing women’s enrollment in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM), some women are successful in these counter-stereotypic disciplines. The present research extended work primarily conducted in the United States by investigating implicit gender-STEM stereotypes—and their relation to performance—among female and male engineering and humanities students in Southern France. In study 1 (N = 55), we tested whether implicit gender-math stereotypes—as measured by the Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald et al. 1998)—would be weaker among female engineering students as compared to female humanities, male engineering and male humanities students. In study 2 (N = 201), we tested whether this same results pattern would be observed with implicit gender-reasoning stereotypes (using a newly created IAT) and, in addition, whether implicit gender-reasoning stereotypes would be more strongly (and negatively) related to math grades for female humanities students as compared to the three other groups. Results showed that female engineering students held weaker implicit gender-math and gender-reasoning stereotypes than female humanities, male engineering and male humanities students. Moreover, implicit stereotyping was more negatively related to math grades for female humanities students than for the three other groups. Together, findings demonstrate that female engineering students hold weaker implicit gender-STEM stereotypes than other groups of students and, in addition, that these stereotypes are not necessarily negatively associated with math performance for all women. Discussion emphasizes how the present research helps refine previous findings and their importance for women’s experience in STEM.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aerospace Institution of Higher Education. (2009). Les formations [Higher education programs]. Retrieved from http://www.isae.fr/fr/index.html.

  • Blanton, H., & Jaccard, J. (2006). Arbitrary metrics in psychology. American Psychologist, 61, 27–41. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.1.27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brauer, M., & McClelland, G. H. (2005). L’utilisation des contrastes dans l’analyse de données: Comment tester des hypothèses spécifiques dans la recherche en psychologie ? [The use of constrasts in data analysis: How to test specific hypotheses in psychological research ?]. L’Année Psychologique, 105, 273–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cejka, M. A., & Eagly, A. H. (1999). Gender-stereotypic images of occupations correspond to the sex segregation of employment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 413–423. doi:10.1177/0146167299025004002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheryan, S., & Plaut, V. C. (2010). Explaining underrepresentation: A theory of precluded interest. Sex Roles, 63, 475–488. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9835-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Crisp, R. J., Bache, L. M., & Maitner, A. T. (2009). Dynamics of social comparison in counter-stereotypic domains: Stereotype boost, not stereotype threat, for women engineering majors. Social Influence, 4, 171–184. doi:10.1080/15534510802607953.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croizet, J.-C., Désert, M., Dutrévis, M., & Leyens, J.-P. (2001). Stereotype threat, social class, gender, and academic under-achievement: When our reputation catches up to us and takes over. Social Psychology of Education, 4, 295–310. doi:10.1023/A:1011336821053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darnon, C., Dompnier, B., Delmas, F., Pulfrey, C., & Butera, F. (2009). Achievement goal promotion at university: Social desirability and social utility of mastery and performance goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 119–134. doi:10.1037/a0012824.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dasgupta, N., & Asgari, S. (2004). Seeing is believing: Exposure to counterstereotypic women leaders and its effect on the malleability of automatic gender stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 642–658. doi:10.1016/j.jesp. 2004.02.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, P. G., Spencer, S. J., Quinn, D., & Gerhardstein, R. (2002). Consuming images: How television commercials that elicit stereotype threat can restrain women academically and professionally. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1615–1628. doi:10.1177/014616702237644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Peslouan, G. (1974). Qui sont les femmes ingénieurs en France? [Who are the female engineers in France]. Rouen: Presses Universitaires de Rouen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women’s achievement-related decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 135–171. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1987.tb00781.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., Jacobs, J. E., & Harold, R. D. (1990). Gender role stereotypes, expectancy effects, and parents’ socialization of gender differences. Journal of Social Issues, 46, 183–201. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4560.1990.tb01929.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • European Commission. (2006). She Figures 2006. Retrieved from http://www.kif.nbi.dk/She_Figures_2006.pdf.

  • French Association of Female Engineers. (2006). Les femmes ingénieurs en France [Engineering women in France]. Retrieved from http://www.femmes-ingenieurs.org/offres/file_inline_src/82/82_P_751_1.pdf.

  • Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychological Review, 109, 3–25. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.109.1.3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Sriram, N. (2006). Consequential validity of the Implicit Association Test: Comment on Blanton and Jaccard (2006). American Psychologist, 61, 56–61. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.1.56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guimond, S., & Roussel, L. (2001). Bragging about one’s school grades: Gender stereotyping and students’ perception of their abilities in science, mathematics and language. Social Psychology of Education, 4, 275–293. doi:10.1023/A:1011332704215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). The role of parents and teachers in the development of gender-related math attitudes. Sex Roles, 66, 153–166. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-9996-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, D. F., Benbow, C. P., Geary, D. C., Gur, R. C., Hyde, J. S., & Gernsbacher, M. A. (2007). The science of sex differences in science and mathematics. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 8, 1–51. doi:10.1111/j.1529-1006.2007.00032.x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartman, H., & Hartman, M. (2008). How undergraduate engineering students perceive women’s (and men’s) problems in science, math and engineering. Sex Roles, 58, 251–265. doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9327-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, M. E., Block, C. J., & Martell, R. F. (1995). Sex stereotypes: Do they influence perceptions of managers? Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 10, 237–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369–1385. doi:10.1177/0146167205275613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huguet, P., & Régner, I. (2007). Stereotype threat among schoolgirls in quasi-ordinary classroom circumstances. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 545–560. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huguet, P., & Régner, I. (2009). Counter-stereotypic beliefs in math do not protect school girls from stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1024–1027. doi:10.1016/j.jesp. 2009.04.029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, J. E. (1991). Influence of gender stereotypes on parent and child mathematics attitudes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 518–527. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.83.4.518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., & Hunyady, O. (2005). Antecedents and consequences of system-justifying ideologies. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 260–265. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00377.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2004). A decade of system justification theory: Accumulated evidence of conscious and unconscious bolstering of the status quo. Political Psychology, 25, 881–919. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9221.2004.00402.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., & McClelland, G. H. (1989). Data analysis: A model-comparison approach. San Diego: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., & Russin, A. (2000). Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 871–888. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.871.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kiefer, A. K., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2007a). Implicit stereotypes and women’s math performance: How implicit gender-math stereotypes influence women’s susceptibility to stereotype threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 825–832. doi:10.1016/j.jesp. 2006.08.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kiefer, A. K., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2007b). Implicit stereotypes, gender identification, and math-related outcomes: A prospective study of female college students. Psychological Science, 18, 13–18. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01841.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marry, C. (2004). Les femmes ingénieurs [Female engineers]. Paris: Belin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martens, A., Johns, M., Greenberg, J., & Schimel, J. (2006). Combating stereotype threat: The effect of self-affirmation on women’s intellectual performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 236–243. doi:10.1016/j.jesp. 2005.04.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martinot, D., Bagès, C., & Désert, M. (2012). French children’s awareness of gender stereotypes about mathematics and reading: When girls improve their reputation in math. Sex Roles, 66, 210–219. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-0032-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McConnell, A. R., & Leibold, J. M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 435–442. doi:10.1006/jesp. 2000.1470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ministry of National Education, Youth, and Community Life. (2011). Les étudiants [The students]. Retrieved from http://www.education.gouv.fr/cid57096/reperes-et-references-statistiques.html#Les%20%C3%A9tudiants.

  • Ministry of National Education and Ministry of Higher Education and Research. (2006). Note d'information du 23 août 2006 [Information note from August 23rd, 2006]. Retrieved from http://media.education.gouv.fr/file/24/9/2249.pdf.

  • National Council of French Engineers and Scientists. (2007). Rapport de la 18ème enquête du CNISF [Report from National Council of French Engineers and Scientists’ 18th survey]. Retrieved from http://aaee.ensil.unilim.fr/spip/IMG/pdf/enquete_CNISF_2007.pdf.

  • National Science Foundation. (2009). TABLE C-4. Bachelor’s degrees, by sex and field: 1997–2006. Retrieved from http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/wmpd/tables.cfm.

  • Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 134, 565–584. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math = Male, Me = Female, therefore Math ≠ Me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 44–59. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., Lindners, N. M., Devos, T., Ayala, A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2009). National differences in gender-science stereotypes predict sex differences in science and math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 106, 10593–10597. doi:10.1073_pnas.0809921106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pronin, E., Steele, C. M., & Ross, L. (2004). Identity bifurcation in response to stereotype threat: Women and mathematics. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 152–168. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00088-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richeson, J. A., & Ambady, N. (2001). Who’s in charge? Effects of situational roles on automatic gender bias. Sex Roles, 44, 493–512. doi:10.1023/A:1012242123824.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richeson, J. A., & Ambady, N. (2003). Effects of situational power on automatic racial prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 177–183. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00521-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1985). Contrast analysis: Focused comparisons in the analysis of variance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rossi, A. (1965). Women in science: Why so few? Science, 148, 1196–1203. doi:10.1126/science.148.3674.1196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., & Ashmore, R. D. (2007). Discrimination and the Implicit Association Test. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10, 359–372. doi:10.1177/1368430207078696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudman, L. A., Greenwald, A. G., & McGhee, D. E. (2001). Implicit self-concept and evaluative implicit gender stereotypes: Self and ingroup share desirable traits. Personality and Social Psycholy Bulletin, 27, 1164–1178. doi:10.1177/0146167201279009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnabel, K., Asendorpf, J. B., & Greenwald, A. G. (2007). Using Implicit Association Tests for the assessment of implicit personality self-concept. In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), Handbook of personality theory and testing (pp. 508–528). London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seibt, B., & Förster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat and performance: How self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regulatory foci. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 38–56. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air. American Psychologist, 52, 613–629. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, J. (2003). Children’s gender stereotypes about math: The role of stereotype stratification. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 2587–2606. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb02782.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, J., James, J. B., & Barnett, R. C. (2002). Learning in a man’s world: Examining the perceptions of undergraduate women in male-dominated academic areas. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 46–50. doi:10.1111/1471-6402.00042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffens, M. C., & Jelenec, P. (2011). Separating implicit gender stereotypes regarding math and language: Implicit ability stereotypes are self-serving for boys and men, but not for girls and women. Sex Roles, 64, 324–335. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9924-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffens, M. C., Jelenec, P., & Noack, P. (2010). On the leaky math pipeline: Comparing implicit math-gender stereotypes and math withdrawal in female and male children and adolescents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 947–963. doi:0.1037/a0019920.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stout, J. G., Dasgupta, N., Husinger, M., & McManus, M. A. (2011). Steming the tide: Using ingroup experts to inoculate women’s self-concept in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. doi:10.1037/a0021385.

  • Summers, L. H. (2005, January 14). Remarks at NBER Conference on Diversifying the Science & Engineering Workforce, Cambridge, MA. Retrieved from http://www.president.harvard.edu/speeches/summers_2005/nber.php.

  • Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2003). Parent-child conversations about science: The socialization of gender inequities? Developmental Psychology, 39, 34–47. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tiedemann, J. (2000). Parents’ gender stereotypes and teachers’ beliefs as predictors of children’s concept of their mathematical ability in elementary school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 144–151. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.92.U44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, M. J., & White, G. B. (2006). Implicit and explicit occupational gender stereotypes. Sex Roles, 55, 259–266. doi:10.1007/s11199-006-9078-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Yoon, K. S., Harold, R. D., Arbreton, A. J. A., Freedman-Doan, C., et al. (1997). Change in children’s competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elementary school years: A 3-year study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 451–469. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.89.3.451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Fabrice Gabarrot, Isabelle Régner, and two anonymous Reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. This article was partially written during the author’s postdoctoral stay at Clermont University-University Blaise Pascal.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Annique Smeding.

Appendix

Appendix

  Words used as stimuli on the gender-math (Study 1) and the gender-reasoning (Study 2) implicit association tests. The original stimuli (in French) are provided along with their English translation

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Smeding, A. Women in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM): An Investigation of Their Implicit Gender Stereotypes and Stereotypes’ Connectedness to Math Performance. Sex Roles 67, 617–629 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0209-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0209-4

Keywords

Navigation