Skip to main content
Log in

Teaching Children to Confront Peers’ Sexist Remarks: Implications for Theories of Gender Development and Educational Practice

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Elementary-school children (81 boys, 72 girls, aged 5–10 years) in the Southwest United States were taught to challenge peers’ sexist remarks to (a) improve school climate for gender nontraditional children, (b) decrease children’s gender-typed attitudes, and (c) test hypotheses linking gender identity and peer-directed gender role behaviors. Children either practiced using retorts to peers’ sexist remarks (practice condition) or heard stories about others’ retorts (narrative condition). At pretest, children rarely challenged peers’ sexist remarks. At posttest, children’s challenges were significantly more common in the practice than narrative condition. At the 6-month posttest, data showed intervention effects had become more widespread. Behavioral changes led to decreases in gender-typing of others among girls but not boys.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arthur, A. E., Bigler, R. S., Liben, L. S., Gelman, S. A., & Ruble, D. N. (2008). Gender stereotyping and prejudice in young children: A developmental intergroup perspective. In S. R. Levy & M. Killen (Eds.), Intergroup attitudes and relations in childhood through adulthood, (pp. 66–86). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashton, E. (1983). Measures of play behavior: The influence of sex-role stereotyped children’s books. Sex Roles, 9, 43–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. New York: General Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, W. C., Engelmann, S., & Thomas, D. R. (1975). Teaching 2: Cognitive learning and instruction. Chicago: Science Research Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R. S. (1995). The role of classification skill in moderating environmental influences on children's gender stereotyping: A study of the functional use of gender in the classroom. Child Development, 66, 1072–1087.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R. S. (1999). Psychological interventions designed to counter sexism in children: Empirical limitations and theoretical foundations. In W. B. Swann, J. H. Langlois & L. A. Gilbert (Eds.), Sexism and stereotypes in modern society: The gender science of Janet Taylor Spence, (pp. 129–151). Washington, DC: American Psychological.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1990). The role of attitudes and interventions in gender-schematic processing. Child Development, 61, 1440–1452.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 162–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birenbaum, M. (2002). Assessing self-directed active learning in primary schools. Assessment in Education, 9, 119–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, J. E. O., Berenbaum, S. A., & Liben, L. S. (2009). Gender development. New York: Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannella, G. S. (1993). Learning through social interaction: Shared cognitive experience, negotiation strategies, and joint concept construction for young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 8, 427–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, P. R., Yunger, J. L., & Perry, D. G. (2003). Gender identity and adjustment in middle childhood. Sex Roles, 49, 95–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coolahan, K., Fantuzzo, J., Mendez, J., & McDermott, P. (2000). Preschool peer interactions and readiness to learn: Relationships between classroom peer play and learning behaviors and conduct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 458–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, N. (2003). Understanding children’s atypical gender behavior. APA Monitor, 34, 40.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeRosier, M. E., & Marcus, S. R. (2005). Building friendships and combating bullying: Effectiveness of S.S.GRIN at one-year follow-up. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 140–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dijkstra, J. K., Lindenberg, S., & Veenstra, R. (2008). Beyond the class norm: Bullying behavior of popular adolescents and its relation to peer acceptance and rejection. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1289–1299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egan, S., & Perry, D. G. (2001). Gender identity: A multidimensional analysis with implications for psychosocial adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 37, 451–463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flex, V. C., Fidler, D. S., & Rogers, R. W. (1976). Sex-role stereotypes: Developmental stereotypes and early intervention. Child Development, 47, 998–1007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, V. A., Bigler, R. S., & Catherwood, D. (2004). The variability and flexibility of gender-typed toy play: A close look at children’s behavioral responses to counterstereotypic models. Sex Roles, 51, 371–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child’s environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, M. (1982). Mastery teaching. El Segundo, CA: TIP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J., & Ettema, J. S. (1982). Positive images. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, P. A. (1986). Modification of children’s gender-stereotyped behavior: General issues and research considerations. Sex Roles, 14, 591–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, P. A., & Walsh, P. V. (1991). Modification of children’s gender-stereotyped behavior. Child Development, 62, 338–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killen, M., & Stangor, C. (2001). Children’s social reasoning about inclusion and exclusion in gender and race peer group contexts. Child Development, 72, 174–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Killen, M., Lee-Kim, J., McGlothin, H., & Stangor, C. (2002). How children and adolescents evaluate gender and racial exclusion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(4, Serial No. 271).

  • Kinder, D., & Carnine, D. (1991). Direct Instruction: What it is and what it is becoming. Journal of Behavioral Education, 12, 193–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kowalski, K, & Kanitkar, K. (2003). Ethnicity and gender in the classroom: A naturalistic study. Poster presented at the biennial meeting for the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL, April.

  • Langlois, J. H., & Downs, A. C. (1980). Mothers, fathers, and peers as socialization agents of sex-typed play behaviors in young children. Child Development, 51, 1237–1247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liben, L. S., & Bigler, R. S. (1987). Reformulating children's gender schemata. In L. S. Liben & M. L. Signorella (Eds.), Children's gender schemata (New Directions for Child Development, No. 38, pp. 89-105). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

  • Liben, L. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2002). The developmental course of gender differentiation: Conceptualizing, measuring, and evaluating constructs and pathways. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(2, Serial No. 269).

  • Liben, L. S., Bigler, R. S, Shechner, T, & Arthur, A. E. (2006). Preschoolers’ sex-typing of self and others: Toward coordinated lifespan measures. Poster presented at the Gender Development Research Conference, San Francisco, California, April.

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental Psychology, 24, 755–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., Evans, S., & Wyman, H. (1999). Social cognition on the playground: Children’s beliefs about playing with girls versus boys and their relations to sex segregated play. Journal of Personal and Social Relationships, 16, 751–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2003). Children’s search for gender cues: Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 67–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 903–933.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McInerney, D. M., & McInerney, V. (2006). Educational psychology: Constructing learning. Frenchs Forest, NSW: Pearson Education Australia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1970). Sex-typing and socialization. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s handbook of child psychology, Vol. 2, (pp. 3–72). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murachver, T., Pipe, M. E., Gordon, R., Owens, J. L., & Fivush, R. (1996). Do, show and tell: Children’s event memories acquired through direct experience, observation and stories. Child Development, 67, 3029–3044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. C. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school: What we know and what we can do. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olweus, D. (2005). A useful evaluation design, and effects of the Olweus bullying prevention program. Psychology, Crime & Law, 11, 389–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paley, V. G. (1984). Boys and girls: Superheroes in the doll corner. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollack, W. S. (1998). Real boys: Recruiting our sons from the myths of boyhood. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poteat, V. P. (2007). Peer group socialization of homophobic attitudes and behavior during adolescence. Child Development, 78, 1830–1842.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roopnarine, J. L. (1984). Sex-typed socialization in mixed-age preschool classrooms. Child Development, 55, 1078–1084.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenshine, B. (1979). Content, time and direct instruction. In P. Peterson & H. Walberg (Eds.), Research on teaching: Concepts findings and implications. Berkeley, CA: McCutchan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salmivalli, C. (1999). Participant role approach to school bullying: Implications for interventions. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 453–459.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salmivalli, C., Kaukiainen, A., & Voeten, M. (2005). Anti-bullying intervention: Implementation and outcome. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 465–487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Serbin, L. A., & Unger, R. K. (1986). Social change: Introduction. Sex Roles, 14, 561–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (2005). Children’s learning. American Psychologist, 60, 769–778.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Signorella, M. L. (1987). Gender schemata: Individual differences and context effects. In L. S. Liben & M. L. Signorella (Eds.), New directions for child development: Children’s gender schemata, vol. 38, (pp. 89–105). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, T. R., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technology of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 349–367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thorne, B. (1993). Gender play: Girls and boys in school. New Jersey: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Verhulst, F. C., & Ormel, J. (2009). Childhood-limited versus persistent antisocial behavior: Why do some recover and others do not? The TRAILS study. Journal of Early Adolescence, 20, 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yunger, J. L., Carver, P. R., & Perry, D. G. (2004). Does gender identity influence children’s psychological well-being? Developmental Psychology, 40, 572–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the Head of St. Francis School, Barbara Porter, as well as the many students, parents, teachers, and staff at the school, who made this work possible. The authors are also grateful to Erin Pahlke, and the undergraduate students in the Gender and Racial Attitudes Laboratory at the University of Texas at Austin who assisted in data collection and entry. Finally, the authors thank two anonymous reviewers who provided especially thoughtful and detailed reviews that contributed greatly to improvements in the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rebecca S. Bigler.

Additional information

This work was supported by a grant from Teaching Tolerance. Portions of the work were presented at the Gender Development Research Conference, April 2006, San Francisco, CA. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Lindsay Lamb, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, 1 University Station D5800, Austin TX 78712, E-mail: llamb@mail.utexas.edu

Appendices

Appendix A

Sexist Vignettes Task

Experimenter says: We are interested in what children do at school with their friends. We are going to ask you some questions about what you would do in some situations. You have to pretend that you are really in the story and then tell me what you think you would really do in that situation. So let’s practice…Pretend that you are in school and a child in your class needs a pencil. You have four extra pencils. She asks you for a pencil. What would you do? Now pretend you are in school and you find 10 cents on the floor in the hall. No one else is in the hall. What would you do?

Great. Let’s do a few more. For each one, pretend that you are at school and the children in the story are your classmates.

Comments About Counter-stereotypic Characteristics

Male target

Pretend that Todd came to school wearing a pink shirt that he had been given by his aunt for his birthday and that his mother made him wear. At lunch that day, one of your classmates told him he was wearing a girl’s shirt. What would you do in this situation?

Female target

Pretend that Sarah got a new haircut. Her hair was cut much shorter than usual and above her ears. When she came to school the next day, one of her classmates asked her, “Why do you have a boy’s haircut?” What would you do in this situation?

Self target

Pretend that you decide to play with the boys [girls] at recess one day. Later that day, none of the girls [boys] in your class will sit with you at lunch because they say you are now a boy [girl]. What would you do in this situation?

Comparative Judgments

Male target

Pretend that Jacob wanted to learn how to do ballet. He started taking ballet classes. The girls in the class told him he would never be very good no matter how hard he practiced because girls are better ballet dancers than boys. What would you do in this situation?

Female target

Pretend that Jenny wanted to play baseball. She decided to join the baseball team. When she arrived at practice the first day, one of her teammates told her she would not be any good because only boys know how to play baseball well. What would you do in this situation?

Self target

Pretend that your class takes a fieldtrip to an old-fashioned town. Some kids get to take lessons about how to make butter and some kids get to take lessons about how to make horseshoes. Some kids tell you that you should take lessons about how to make horseshoes [or make butter] because boys are better than girls at working with metal and girls are better than boys at cooking. What would you do in this situation?

Exclusion

Male target

Pretend that a group of girls was building a sandcastle in the sandbox. Ryan wanted to help them. The girls told Ryan that he wasn’t allowed to help because the sandbox was only for girls that day. What would you do in this situation?

Female target

Pretend that a group of boys were swinging on the tire swing at recess. Amanda wanted to swing and asked the boys if she could join them. They told her she wasn’t allowed because only boys could use the swing that day. What would you do in this situation?

Self target

Pretend you are playing in the park. A group of boys [or girls] are building a clubhouse. They tell you that you cannot build with them because the clubhouse is only for boys [or girls]. What would you do in this situation?

Role Stereotyping

Male target

Pretend that Emily becomes sick during class. The teacher asks for a volunteer to take her to the nurse’s office. Rachel says that a girl should take Emily because girls are good at helping sick people. What would you do in this situation?

Female target

Pretend that Tom and Jane were talking about what they wanted to be when they grew up. Jane said she wanted to be the President of the United States. Tom told her that she couldn’t because only boys can be President of the United States. What would you do in this situation?

Self target

Pretend that you are helping your parents cook dinner for friends. Your dad is grilling and your mom is baking a cake in the kitchen. Your friend tells you that you are a boy [or girl] so you have to help your dad grill [help your mom bake the cake] because this is a job for boys [or girls]. What would you do in this situation?

Highlighting Gender in a Neutral Context

Pretend that your class is going to the library. Your teacher tells your class to line up boy/girl/boy/girl at the door. What would you do in this situation?

Appendix B

Means and Standard Deviations for Children’s Gender Identity and Gender-typing at Pretest

 

Practice (n = 31)

Narrative (n = 30)

Pretest Measures

M

SD

M

SD

Self (P/COAT-PM)

 Masculine

.59

.12

.59

.10

 Feminine

.69

.13

.59

.10

Other (P/COAT-AM)

 Masculine

.63

.26

.60

.24

 Feminine

.71

.40

.59

.10

Gender Identity

 Gender Typicality

2.84

.69

2.76

.68

 Felt Pressure

2.18

.67

2.14

.72

 Gender Contentedness

2.93

.63

2.78

.76

Note. Gender-typing of the self scores are the proportions of items that participants reported being highly interested in performing and thus scores range from .00 to 1.00. Gender-typing of other scores are the proportion of “both men and women” responses and thus scores range from .00 to 1.00. Gender identity scores range from 1 to 4, with higher scores indicating stronger endorsement of the construct.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lamb, L.M., Bigler, R.S., Liben, L.S. et al. Teaching Children to Confront Peers’ Sexist Remarks: Implications for Theories of Gender Development and Educational Practice. Sex Roles 61, 361–382 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-009-9634-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-009-9634-4

Keywords

Navigation