Skip to main content
Log in

Connecting goals and actions during reading: the role of illustrations

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The ability to integrate information that is separated within a text, such as connecting a character’s action to a goal stated earlier in the text, is a critical factor in narrative comprehension. In the present study, we analyze the ability of 9- and 11-year olds to integrate such information. In addition, we examined the effect of illustrations on integration process by using on-line comprehension measures. Participants read narratives that conveyed a character’s goal early in the text, and an action was described later on that was either consistent or inconsistent with the goal. Narratives were presented either with an illustration that mirrored the situation described in the goal, or without an illustration. The results show that 11-year-olds spent more time on the inconsistent actions than on the consistent actions; in addition, 9-year-olds detected the inconsistency when illustrations were provided. An additional finding was that working memory moderated children’s ability to connect actions and goals. These results suggest that children as young as 9 years form connections between characters’ actions and their goals during text processing, although they need an illustration to keep the goal information activated to detect the inconsistency. Illustrations’ effectiveness was interpreted in terms of extra processing of the characters’ goal.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Baddeley (2000) also suggested an additional component of the model, the episodic buffer, which integrates and temporarily stores information from the articulatory loop and the visual-spatial sketchpad.

  2. The authors thank an anonymous reviewer who suggested this issue.

References

  • Ackerman, B. P. (1984). Storage and processing constraints on integrating story Information in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 64–92. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(84)90019-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ackerman, B. P., & McGraw, M. (1991). Constraints on the causal inferences of children and adults in comprehending stories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 364–394. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(91)90083-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Albrecht, J. E., & Myers, J. L. (1995). Role of context in accessing distant information during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1459–1468. doi:10.1037//0278-7393.21.6.1459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1996). Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 5–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, M. A., Faulkner, H., Wilkinson, M., & Dennis, M. (2004). Meaning construction and integration in children with hydrocephalus. Brain and Language, 89, 47–56. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00295-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004a). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31–42. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Lemmon, K. (2004b). Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: The influence of reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 671–681. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carney, R. N., & Levin, J. R. (2002). Pictorial illustrations still improve students learning from text. Educational Psychology Review, 14, 5–26. doi:10.1023/A:1013176309260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1992). The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casteel, M. A. (1993). Effects of inference necessity and reading goal on children’s inferential generation. Developmental Psychology, 29, 346–357. doi:10.1037//0012-1649.29.2.346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuetos, F., Rodríguez, B., Ruano, E., & Arribas, D. (2007). PROLEC-R, Batería de evaluación de los procesos lectores [PROLEC-R, Assessment battery of reading processes]. Madrid: TEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(4), 450–466. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, T. M., & Cunningham, D. J. (1996). Constructivism: Implications for the design and delivery of instruction. In D. H. Jonassen (Ed.), Handbook of research for educational communications and technology (pp. 170–198). New York: Macmillan Library Reference USA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutke, S., & Rinck, M. (2006). Multimedia learning: Working memory and the learning of word and picture diagrams. Learning and Instruction, 16, 526–537. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.10.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Egidi, G., & Gerrig, R. J. (2006). Readers’ experiences of characters’ goals and actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1322–1329. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.1322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, C. R., & Bloom, C. P. (1988). Causal reasoning in the comprehension of simple narrative texts. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 235–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gambrell, L. B., & Jawitz, P. B. (1993). Mental imagery, text illustrations, and children’s story comprehension and recall. Reading Research Quarterly, 28(3), 264–276. doi:10.2307/747998.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., & Langston, W. E. (1992). Comprehension of illustrated text: Pictures help to build mental models. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 129–151. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(92)90008-L.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Meyer, M., & Lindem, K. (1987). Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 69–83. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(87)90063-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, S. R., & Varnhagen, C. K. (1986). Memory for embedded and sequential story structures. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 401–418. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(86)90034-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371–395. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.3.371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gyselinck, V., Cornoldi, C., Dubois, V., De Beni, R., & Ehrlich, M. F. (2002). Visuospatial memory and phonological loop in learning from multimedia. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 665–685. doi:10.1002/acp.823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gyselinck, V., & Tardieu, H. (1999). The role of illustrations in text comprehension: What, when, for whom, and why? In H. Van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 195–218). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huitema, J. S., Dopkins, S., Klin, C. M., & Myers, J. L. (1993). Connecting goals and actions during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1053–1060. doi:10.1037//0278-7393.19.5.1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., van den Broek, P., Espin, C., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Sources of individual differences in reading comprehension and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 719–729. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levie, W., & Lentz, R. (1982). Effects of text illustrations: A review of research. Educational Communications and Technology Journal, 30(4), 195–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, J. R., Anglin, G. J., & Carney, R. N. (1987). On empirically validating functions of pictures in prose. In D. M. Willows & H. A. Houghton (Eds.), The psychology of illustration: I. Basic research (pp. 51–85). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, M. F., & Radvansky, G. A. (1997). The fate of completed goal information in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 293–310. doi:10.1006/jmla.1996.2491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, J. S., & van den Broek, P. (2007). Understanding the glue of narrative structure: Children’son- and off-line inferences about characters’ goals. Cognitive Development, 22, 323–340. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.02.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magliano, J. P., Zwaan, R. A., & Graesser, A. C. (1999). The role of situational continuity in narrative understanding. In H. E. van Oostendorp & S. R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representations during reading (pp. 219–245). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, N., Cooper, M., & Sweller, J. (1996). Understanding instructions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 49–63. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.88.1.49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., & Gallini, J. K. (1990). When is an illustration worth ten thousand words? Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 715–726. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.82.4.715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, R. E., Heiser, J., & Lonn, S. (2001). Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: When presenting more material results in less understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 187–198. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrudden, M. T., Magliano, J. P., & Schraw, G. (2011). The effect of diagrams on online reading processes and memory. Discourse Processes, 48, 69–92. doi:10.1080/01638531003694561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrudden, M. T., Schraw, G., & Lehman, S. (2009). The use of adjunct displays to facilitate comprehension of causal relationships in expository text. Instructional Science, 37, 65–86. doi:10.1007/s11251-007-9036-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Múñez, D., Orrantia, J., & Rosales, J. (2013). The effect of external representations on compare word problems: Supporting mental model construction. The Journal of Experimental Education. doi:10.1080/00220973.2012.715095.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., Adams, J. W., Bowyer-Crane, C. A., & Snowling, M. J. (1999). Working memory deficits in poor comprehenders reflect underlying language impairments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 139–158. doi:10.1006/jecp.1999.2498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J., Hartt, J., & Samols, D. (2005). Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Reading and Writing, 18, 657–686. doi:10.1007/s11145-005-3355-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pike, M. M., Barnes, M. A., & Barron, R. W. (2010). The role of illustrations in children’s inferential comprehension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 243–255. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2009.10.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poynor, D. V., & Morris, R. K. (2003). Inferred goals in narratives: Evidence from self-paced reading, recall, and eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 3–9. doi:10.1037//0278-7393.29.1.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radvansky, G. A., & Curiel, J. C. (1998). Narrative comprehension and aging: The fate of completed goal information. Psychology and Aging, 13, 69–79. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.13.1.69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sadoski, M. (1985). The natural use of imagery in story comprehension and recall: Replication and extension. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 658–667. doi:10.2307/747949.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez, E., García, J. R., & Gonzalez, A. J. (2007). Can differences in the ability to recognize words cease to have and effect under certain reading conditions? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40(4), 290–306. doi:10.1177/00222194070400040101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, M., Andrusiak, P., Reisdorf, P., & Black, N. L. (1992). Individual differences in bridging inference processes. Memory & Cognition, 20, 539–548. doi:10.3758/BF03199586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, M., & Halldorson, M. (1996). Constructing and validating motive bridging inferences. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 1–38. doi:10.1006/cogp.1996.0001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. G. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In R. O. Freedle (Ed.), New directions in discourse processing (pp. 53–120). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suh, S. Y., & Trabasso, T. (1993). Inferences during reading: Converging evidence from discourse analysis, talk-aloud protocols, and recognition priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 279–300. doi:10.1006/jmla.1993.1015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trabasso, T., & Nickels, M. (1992). The development of goal plans of action in the narration of a picture story. Discourse Processes, 15, 249–275. doi:10.1080/01638539209544812.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trabasso, T., & Sperry, L. (1985). Causal relatedness and importance of story events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 595–611. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(85)90048-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trabasso, T., & van den Broek, P. (1985). Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 612–630. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(85)90049-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trabasso, T., van den Broek, P., & Suh, S. Y. (1989). Logical necessity and transitivity of causal relations in stories. Discourse Processes, 12, 1–25. doi:10.1080/01638538909544717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P. (1989). Causal reasoning and inference making in judging the importance of story statements. Child Development, 60, 286–297. doi:10.2307/1130976.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P. (1994). Comprehension and memory for narrative texts: Inferences and coherence. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 539–588). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P. (1997). Discovering the cement of the universe: The development of event comprehension from childhood to adulthood. In P. van den Broek, P. J. Bauer, & T. Bourg (Eds.), Developmental spans in event comprehension and representation: Bridging fictional and actual events (pp. 321–343). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., Kremer, K., Lynch, J. S., Butler, J., White, M. J., et al. (2005a). Assessment of comprehension abilities in young children. In S. Stahl & S. Paris (Eds.), Children’s reading comprehension and assessment (pp. 107–130). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P., & Lorch, R. F. (1993). Network representations of causal relations in memory for narrative texts: Evidence from primed recognition. Discourse Processes, 16, 75–98. doi:10.1080/01638539309544830.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P., Lynch, J. S., Naslund, J., Ievers-Landis, C. E., & Verduin, K. (2003). The development of comprehension of main ideas in narratives: Evidence from the selection of titles. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 707–718. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Broek, P., Rapp, D. N., & Kendeou, P. (2005b). Integrating memory-based and constructionist processes in accounts of reading comprehension. Discourse Processes, 39, 299–316. doi:10.1207/s15326950dp3902&3_11.

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Schoot, M., Reijntjes, A., & van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (2012). How do children deal with inconsistencies in text? An eye fixation and self-paced reading study in good and poor reading comprehenders. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 1665–1690. doi:10.1007/s11145-011-9337-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Oostendorp, H. E., & Goldman, S. R. (1999). The construction of mental representations during reading. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2005). Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para niños-IV [The Wechsler intelligence scale for children-fourth edition]. Madrid: TEA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A., Langston, M. C., & Graesser, A. C. (1995). The construction of situation models in narrative comprehension: An event indexing model. Psychological Science, 6, 292–297. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00513.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162–185. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.123.2.162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Grant PSI2011-27737 to J. Orrantia from the Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación in Spain.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Josetxu Orrantia.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Orrantia, J., Múñez, D. & Tarín, J. Connecting goals and actions during reading: the role of illustrations. Read Writ 27, 153–170 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9437-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-013-9437-4

Keywords

Navigation