Skip to main content
Log in

Using eye and pen movements to trace the development of writing expertise: case studies of a 7th, 9th and 12th grader, graduate student, and professional writer

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study was designed to enhance our understanding of the changing relationship between low- and high-level writing processes in the course of development. A dual description of writing processes was undertaken, based on (a) the respective time courses of these processes, as assessed by an analysis of eye and pen movements, and (b) the semantic characteristics of the writers’ scripts. To conduct a more fine-grained description of processing strategies, a “case study” approach was adopted, whereby a comprehensive range of measures was used to assess processes within five writers with different levels of expertise. The task was to continue writing a story based on excerpt from a source document (incipit). The main results showed two developmental patterns linked to expertise: (a) a gradual acceleration in low- and high-level processing (pauses, flow), associated with (b) changes in the way the previous text was (re)read.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Expressed in milliseconds, the minimum value for pause thresholds is determined by the formula 3 × (1,000/sampling rate). For example, with a 200 Hz sampling tablet, i.e., sampling 200 pen positions per second (=1,000 ms), the minimum threshold will be: 3 × (1,000/200) = 15 ms. As the "loss" of one or two consecutive data samples may occur during the acquisition or transmission of the data to the programme Eye and Pen imposes a minimum value equal to the duration of three samples.

  2. Latent semantic analysis (LSA) is a theory and method for extracting and representing the contextual-usage meaning of words by statistical computations applied to a large corpus of text (Foltz, Kintsch, & Landauer, 1998). The underlying idea is that the totality of information about all the word contexts in which a given word does and does not appear provides a set of mutual constraints that largely determines the similarity of meaning of words and set of words to each other (see http://lsa.colorado.edu/).

  3. Here, a fixation was coded “outside” each time it lasted longer than 100 ms and occurred in a non-informative zone of the screen (neither on the incipit, nor on the text produced so far).

References

  • Alamargot, D., & Chanquoy, L. (2001). Through the models of writing. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alamargot, D., Chanquoy, L., & Chuy, M. (2005a). L’élaboration du contenu du texte: De la mémoire à long terme à l’environnement de la tâche. Psychologie Française, 50(3), 287–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alamargot, D., Chesnet, D., Dansac, C., & Ros, C. (2006). Eye and pen: A new device to study the reading during writing. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 38(2), 287–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alamargot, D., Dansac, C., Chesnet, D., & Fayol, M. (2007). Parallel processing before and after pauses: A combined analysis of graphomotor and eye movements during procedural text production. In M. Torrance, L. van Waes, & D. Galbraith (Eds.), Writing and cognition. Research and applications (Vol. 20, pp. 13–29). Dordrecht: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alamargot, D., Dansac, C., Ros, C., & Chuy, M. (2005b). Rédiger un texte procédural à partir de sources: Relations entre l’empan de mémoire de travail et l’activité oculaire du scripteur [Composing a procedural text from sources: Relationships between working memory span and writer’s eye movements]. In D. Alamargot, P. Terrier, & J. M. Cellier (Eds.), Production, compréhension et usages des écrits techniques au travail (pp. 51–68). Toulouse: Octarès.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alamargot, D., & Fayol, M. (2009). Modeling the development of written composition. In R. Beard, D. Myhill, M. Nystrand & J. Riley (Eds.), Handbook of writing development (616 pp). London: Sage.

  • Alamargot, D., & Lebrave, J. L. (in press). A mutual contribution by cognitive psychology and genetic criticism to the study of professional writing. European Psychologist.

  • Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersson, B., Holmqvist, K., Holsanova, J., Johansson, V., Karlsson, H., Strömqvist, S., et al. (2006). Combining keystroke logging with eye-tracking. In L. Van Waes, M. Leijten, C. Neuwirth, et al. (Eds.), Writing and digital media (Vol. 17, pp. 166–172). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baccino, T., & Pynte, J. (1998). Spatial encoding and referential processing during reading. European Psychologist, 3(1), 51–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C., Burtis, P. J., & Scardamalia, M. (1988). Cognitive operations in constructing main points in written composition. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 261–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1983). Levels of inquiry in writing research. In P. Mosenthal, S. Walmsley, & L. Tamor (Eds.), Research on writing: Principles and method (pp. 3–25). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written composition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. (1994). Reading and writing acquisition: A developmental neuropsychological perspective. Madison, WI: WCB Brown & Benchmark. (Reprinted 1996, Westview Press, Boulder, CO. Distributed by the Psychological Corporation).

  • Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Jones, J., Wolf, B., Gould, L., Anderson-Youngstrom, M., et al. (2006). Early development of language by hand: Composing-, reading-, listening-, and speaking-connections, three letter writing modes, and fast mapping in spelling. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 61–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., & Swanson, H. L. (1994). Modifying hayes and flower model of skilled writing to explain beginning and developing writing. In E. C. Butterfield (Ed.), Advances in cognition and educational practice (Vol. 2: Children’s writing: Toward a process theory of development of skilled writing) (pp. 57–82). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binder, K. S., Duffy, S. A., & Rayner, K. (2001). The effects of thematic fit and discourse context on syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(2), 297–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterfield, E. C., Hacker, D. J., & Albertson, L. R. (1996). Environmental, cognitive and metacognitive influences on text revision: Assessing the evidence. Educational Psychology Review, 8(3), 239–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caccamise, D. J. (1987). Idea generation in writing. In A. Matsuhashi (Ed.), Writing in real time: Modeling production processes (pp. 224–253). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caporossi, G., Alamargot, D., & Chesnet, D. (2004). Using the computer to study the dynamics of handwriting processes. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 3245, 242–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chesnet, D., & Alamargot, D. (2005). Analyses en temps réel des activités oculaires et graphomotrices du scripteur: Intérêt du dispositif ‘eye and pen’ [Real time analysis of the writer graphomotoric and ocular activities: interests of the ‘eye and pen’ device]. L’Année Psychologique, 105(3), 477–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A. (1968) Belle du Seigneur [Her Lover]. Paris: Gallimard.

  • Coirier, P., Andriessen, J., & Chanquoy, L. (1999). From planning to translating: The specificity of argumentative writing. In J. Andriessen & P. Coirier (Eds.), Foundations of argumentative text processing (pp. 1–28). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

  • Ehrlichman, H. (1981). From gaze aversion to eye-movement suppression: An investigation of the cognitive interference explanation of gaze patterns during conversation. British Journal of Social Psychology, 20, 233–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrlichman, H., & Weinberger, A. (1978). Lateral eye movements and hemispheric asymmetry: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 1080–1101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long term working memory. Psychological Review, 102(2), 211–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fayol, M. (1999). From on-line management problems to strategies in written production. In M. Torrance & G. C. Jeffery (Eds.), The cognitive demands of writing: Processing capacity and working memory effects in text production (pp. 13–23). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foltz, P. W., Kintsch, W., & Landauer, T. K. (1998). The measurement of textual coherence with latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 285–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Schroeder, J. L., & Robertson, D. A. (1998). Averting the gaze disengages the environment and facilitates remembering. Memory and Cognition, 26(4), 651–658.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, D. J. (1994). Comprehension monitoring as a writing process. In E. C. Butterfield & J. S. Carlson (Eds.), Advances in cognition and educational practice. Children’s writing: Toward a process theory of the development of skilled writing (Vol. 6, pp. 143–172). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. R. (1996). A new framework for understanding cognition and affect in writing. In C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences and applications (pp. 1–27). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. R., & Flower, L. S. (1980). Identifying the organization of writing processes. In L. W. Gregg & E. R. Steinberg (Eds.), Cognitive processes in writing (pp. 3–30). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heurley, L., & Ganier, F. (2002). La production de textes techniques écrits [The production of technical texts]. In M. Fayol (Ed.), Production du langage: Traité des sciences cognitives (pp. 229–249). Paris: Hermès-Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyönä, J., Lorch, R. F., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2002). Individual differences in reading to summarize expository text: Evidence from eye fixations patterns. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(1), 44–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 4, 329–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99(1), 122–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg, R. T. (1987). Effects of topic knowledge on the allocation of processing time and cognitive effort to writing processes. Memory and Cognition, 15(3), 256–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg, R. T. (2001). Long-term working memory in text production. Memory & Cognition, 29(1), 43–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg, R. T. (2008). Training writing skills: A cognitive development perspective. Journal of Writing Research, 1(1), 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, C. M., Marek, J. P., & Lea, J. (1996). Concurrent and retrospective protocols in writing research. In G. Rijlaarsdam, H. van den Bergh, & M. Couzijn (Eds.), Theories, models and methodology in writing research (pp. 542–556). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, C. M., & Ransdell, S. (Eds.). (1996). The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacArthur, C., Graham, N. L., & Fitzgerald, J. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of writing research. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCutchen, D. (1996). A capacity theory of writing: Working memory in composition. Educational Psychology Review, 8(3), 299–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myhill, D., & Locke, T. (2007). Editorial: Composition in the English/literacy classroom. English Teaching: Practice and Critique, 6(1), 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ : Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nottbusch, G. (2009). Grammatical planning, execution and control in written sentence production. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal (this issue).

  • Plane, S., Alamargot, D., & Lebrave, J. L. (in press). Temporalité de l’écriture et rôle du texte produit dans l’activité rédactionnelle [The written temporality and the role of the text produced so far during text composition]. Langages.

  • Quinlan, T., & Alamargot, D. (2007). Composing from sources: Effects of genre and of working memory capacities. In D. Alamargot, P. Terrier, & J. M. Cellier (Eds.), Written documents in the workplace (pp. 61–74). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1991). Literate expertise. In K. A. Ericsson & J. Smith (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 172–194). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H. L., & Berninger, V. W. (1996). Individual differences in children’s working memory and writing skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 358–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, M. (1996). Is writing expertise like other kinds of expertise? In G. Rijlaarsdam, H. van den Bergh, & M. Couzijn (Eds.), Theories, models and methodology in writing research (pp. 3–9). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van den Bergh, H., & Rijlaarsdam, G. (1996). The dynamics of composing: Modeling writing process data. In C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 207–232). Mahwah: Laurence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Geest, T. (1996). Studying “real life” writing processes: A proposal and an example. In C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of writing. Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. 309–322). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Waes, L., Leijten, M., & Quinlan, T. (2009). Reading during sentence composing and error correction: A multilevel analysis of the influences of task complexity. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal (this issue).

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Nadia Rodier (educator in “France Bloch Sarrazin” college) for her help in the experiment management, Marie-Françoise Crété for the editing and Elizabeth Portier for her translation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Denis Alamargot.

Appendices

Appendix

Incipit: excerpt taken from Belle du Seigneur [Her Lover], by Albert Cohen (175 words)

French version

Aude se dit en passant devant la bibliothèque qu’une porte en somme la séparait de l’homme et qu’il fallait à tout prix avoir une explication. En finir, mon Dieu! Elle en avait assez. Certainement il l’avait suivie ce matin. Assez d’être persécutée par ce sourire. Elle lui ferait comprendre qu’il devait partir, qu’il était ignoble de jouer ainsi avec Jacques et Adrienne. Angoissée et sentant qu’elle avait tort, elle poussa la porte, éprouvant sur le seuil la délectation du vertige et peut-être l’affreuse joie de suivre la mauvaise voie destinée de toute éternité.

  • Je vous dérange.

  • Quoi? demanda-t-il avec hargne ahurissement, distraction et génialité.

  • Je vous dérange.

  • Oui oui merci.

Elle s’approcha des rayons, fit une pile de livres qui tombèrent.

  • Vous avez terminé vos recherches bibliographiques? demanda-t-il gravement, en faisant avec la cordelière de sa robe moirée un mouvement menaçant de fronde.

Elle chercha en vain une phrase insolente et s’avança sans savoir ce qu’elle allait dire.

English version

As she walked past the library, Aude told herself that only a door stood between her and the man she absolutely needed to have it out with. How she longed to get it over and done with! She had enough—enough of being persecuted by that smile. He had definitely followed her that morning. She would make it quite clear to him that he had to leave, that it was simply appalling to toy with Jacques and Adrienne in that fashion. With mounting trepidation and the uneasy feeling that she was making a mistake, she pushed the door open. As she crossed the threshold, she experienced an intoxicating thrill and perhaps, too, the dreadful joy of following the wrong path as she had always been destined to do.

  • ‘I’m disturbing you.’

  • ‘What?’ he asked, with a mixture of spite, stupefaction, absent-mindedness and consummate cleverness.

  • ‘I’m disturbing you.’

  • ‘Yes, yes, please do.’

  • She went up to the shelves and made a pile of books, which promptly collapsed.

  • ‘You’ve finished your bibliographic research?’ he inquired gravely, giving a mutinous tug to the cord of his moiré dressing gown.

  • She cast around in vain for an insolent rejoinder and moved forward, with absolutely no idea of what she was going to say.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Alamargot, D., Plane, S., Lambert, E. et al. Using eye and pen movements to trace the development of writing expertise: case studies of a 7th, 9th and 12th grader, graduate student, and professional writer. Read Writ 23, 853–888 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9191-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9191-9

Keywords

Navigation