Skip to main content
Log in

A not-so-simple view of reading: how oral vocabulary and visual-word recognition complicate the story

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study sought to clarify the relations amongst serial decoding, irregular word recognition, listening comprehension, facets of oral vocabulary and reading comprehension in two cohorts of children differing in reading level. In the process, the components of the simple view of reading were evaluated. Students in grades 1 (n = 67) and 6 (n = 56) were assessed on measures of phonological awareness, decoding, irregular word recognition, listening comprehension, oral vocabulary, and reading comprehension. Even when all other measures were controlled, vocabulary was found to explain reading comprehension in grade 6 but not grade 1. Vocabulary also predicted decoding in grade 6 and irregular word recognition in both grades. These results are interpreted as supporting a not-so-simple view of the constructs underlying reading comprehension that acknowledges complex connections between print skills and oral language.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The same pattern of results was found if the decoding composite was replaced with only the WRMT Word Attack score.

References

  • Adams, M. J., & Huggins, A. W. F. (1985). The growth of children’s sight vocabulary: A quick test with educational and theoretical implications. Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 262–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L. (2002). Bringing words to life; Robust vocabulary instruction. New York, NY: Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A., & Boote, C. (2006). An effective method for building meaning vocabulary in primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 44–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Braze, D., Tabor, W., Shankweiler, D. P., & Mencl, W. E. (2007). Speaking up for vocabulary: Reading skill differences in young adults. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 226–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (1999). Inference making ability and its relation to comprehension failure in young children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11, 489–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (2005). Modeling reading: The dual-route approach. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 6–23). Oxford, England: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A., & Stanovich, K. (1991). Tracking the unique effects of print exposure in children: Associations with vocabulary, general knowledge and spelling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 264–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (2006). Peabody picture vocabulary test—4. Circle Pins, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. (2005). Learning to read: Theory, findings and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P., & Tunmer, W. (1986). Decoding, reading and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagtvet, B. (2003). Listening comprehension and reading comprehension in poor decoders: Evidence for the importance of syntactic and semantic skills as well as phonological skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 505–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M., & Seidenberg, M. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in meaning: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111, 662–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, T. C., & Kirby, J. R. (2006). The contribution of naming speed to the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 339–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J. R., & Savage, R. S. (2008). Can the simple view deal with the complexities of reading? Literacy, 42, 75–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landi, N., & Perfetti, C. (2007). An electrophysiological investigation of semantic and phonological processing in skilled and less-skilled comprehenders. Brain and Language, 102, 30–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 40, 665–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (1998). Semantic processing and the development of word-recognition skills: Evidence from children with developmental reading comprehension difficulties. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 85–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (1999). Developmental differences in sensitivity to semantic relations among good and poor comprehenders: Evidence from semantic priming. Cognition, 90, B1–B13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 342–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakhill, J., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 443–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, G. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 554–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 357–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbrow, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (Vol. 11, pp. 67–86). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Sideridis, G., & Mouzaki, A. (2007). Development of lexical mediation in the relationship between reading comprehension and word reading skills in Greek. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 165–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricketts, J., Nation, K., & Bishop, D. (2007). Vocabulary is important for some, but not all reading skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 235–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savage, R. (2006). Reading comprehension is not always the product of decoding and listening comprehension: Evidence from teenagers who are very poor readers. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 143–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M., & McClelland, J. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals—fourth edition (CELF-4). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Senechal, M., Ouellette, G., & Rodney, D. (2006). The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary to future reading. In S. B. Neuman & D. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (Vol. 2). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storch, S., & Whitehurst, G. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934–947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tannenbaum, K. R., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (2006). Relationships between word knowledge and reading comprehension in their-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 381–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Tunmer, W. E., Jaccard, J. J., & Chen, R. (2007). Components of reading ability: Multivariate evidence for a convergent skills model of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 3–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L., & van Leeuwe, J. (2008). Prediction of the development of reading comprehension: A longitudinal study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 407–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R., Torgeseon, J., & Rashotte, C. (1999). TOWRE: Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walley, A., Metsala, J., & Garlock, V. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phonological awareness and early reading ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 5–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (1992). TOWK: Test of word knowledge. Toronto, Canada: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. (1998). WRMT-R: Woodcock reading mastery tests—revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was funded in part by a grant to the first author from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC). We sincerely thank the children, parents, teachers, administrators and school personnel of NB District 2. We also acknowledge the helpful comments of a reviewer that served to highlight the distinction between constructs and measures discussed here. We also thank Jill Fraser and Angela Dalton for their help and support and Katrina Ouellette for overseeing all things lab related.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gene Ouellette.

Appendices

Appendix A

Table 6 Irregular word list

Appendix B

Table 7 Definitions list

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ouellette, G., Beers, A. A not-so-simple view of reading: how oral vocabulary and visual-word recognition complicate the story. Read Writ 23, 189–208 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9159-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9159-1

Keywords

Navigation