Skip to main content
Log in

The stability and reliability of attentional bias measures in the dot-probe task: Evidence from both traditional mean bias scores and trial-level bias scores

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A new method of calculating attentional bias from the dot-probe task measures fluctuations in bias towards and away from emotional stimuli over time using trial level bias score metrics. We assessed the stability and reliability of traditional attentional bias scores and trial level bias score measures of attentional bias across time in two five-block dot-probe task experiments in non-clinical samples. In experiments 1 and 2, both traditional attentional bias scores and trial level bias score measures of attentional bias did not habituate/decrease across time. In general, trial level bias score metrics (i.e., attention bias variability as well as the mean biases toward and away from threat) were more reliable than the traditional attention bias measure. This pattern was observed across both experiments. The traditional bias score, however, did improve in reliability in the later blocks of the fearful face dot-probe task. Although trial level bias score measures did not habituate and were more reliable across blocks, these measures did not correlate with state or trait anxiety. On the other hand, trial level bias score measures were strongly correlated with general reaction time variability—and after controlling for this effect no longer superior in reliability in comparison to the traditional attention bias measure. We conclude that general response variability should be removed from trial level bias score measures to ensure that they truly reflect attention bias variability.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

The data associated with this manuscript has been shared on the Open Science Framework (OSF) in a project called “The stability and reliability of attentional bias measures in the dot-probe task: Evidence from both traditional mean bias scores and trial-level bias scores” https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/9M5NB.

Notes

  1. 128 participants were recruited, but one participant did not have sufficient data for block 1 of the dot-probe task and was therefore excluded.

  2. Fearful face stimuli were: 207F, 208F, 213F, 217F (Gur et al. 2002), AF14AFS, AF19AFS, AF22AFS, AM10AFS, AM22AFS, AM34AFS (Lundqvist et al. 1998); Neutral face stimuli were: 207 N, 213 N, 217 N (Gur et al. 2002), AF14NES, AF19NES, AF22NES, AM10NES, AM22NES, AM34NES (Lundqvist et al. 1998).

  3. Although previous research suggests that there are sex differences in trial level bias score measures of attentional bias (Carlson et al. 2019a), supplementary analyses found no effects of sex in the current dataset (all p ≥ .32). However, this may in part be due to the large percentage of female participants in the current dataset (72%). Thus, participant sex was not included in the analyses reported here.

  4. 86 participants were recruited, but one participant did not have sufficient data for block 1 and 2 of the dot-probe task and three did not have STAI score were therefore excluded.

  5. Threatening IAPS images were: 1030, 1080, 1201, 1300, 1930, 2120, 6200, 6260, 6510, 9440; Neutral IAPS images were: 7000, 7004, 2745.1, 7041, 7002, 2383, 5390, 7006, 7510, 7060.

  6. It should be noted that RT variability in these studies was obtained from a very small number of practice trials, which may not be representative of RT variability during the task.

References

  • Aday, J. S., & Carlson, J. M. (2017). Structural MRI-based measures of neuroplasticity in an extended amygdala network as a target for attention bias modification treatment outcome. Medical Hypotheses, 109, 6–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2017.09.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aday, J. S., & Carlson, J. M. (2019). Extended testing with the dot-probe task increases test–retest reliability and validity. Cognitive Processing, 20, 65–72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-018-0886-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. P. (1988). Anxiety and attentional bias: State and trait. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 165–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Fee, A. L., & Reinke, K. S. (2009). Backward masked snakes and guns modulate spatial attention. Evolutionary Psychology, 7(4), 527–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Aday, J. S., & Rubin, D. (2019a). Temporal dynamics in attention bias: Effects of sex differences, task timing parameters, and stimulus valence. Cognition and Emotion, 33(6), 1271–1276. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1536648.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Lehman, B. R., & Thompson, J. L. (2019b). Climate change images produce an attentional bias associated with pro-environmental disposition. Cognitive Processing. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-019-00902-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., & Mujica-Parodi, L. R. (2015). Facilitated attentional orienting and delayed disengagement to conscious and nonconscious fearful faces. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 39(1), 69–77. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-014-0185-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., & Reinke, K. S. (2014). Attending to the fear in your eyes: Facilitated orienting and delayed disengagement. Cognition and Emotion, 28(8), 1398–1406. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.885410.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Torrence, R. D., & Vander Hyde, M. R. (2016). Beware the eyes behind the mask: The capture and hold of selective attention by backward masked fearful eyes. Motivation and Emotion, 40(3), 498–505. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9542-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, P., Marinovic, W., Todd, J., Basanovic, J., Chen, N. T., & Notebaert, L. (2020). What is attention bias variability? Examining the potential roles of attention control and response time variability in its relationship with anxiety.

  • Cooper, R. M., & Langton, S. R. (2006). Attentional bias to angry faces using the dot-probe task? It depends when you look for it. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1321–1329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. L., Rosenfield, D., Bernstein, A., Zvielli, A., Reinecke, A., Beevers, C. G., et al. (2016). Attention bias dynamics and symptom severity during and following CBT for social anxiety disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 84(9), 795–802. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Draheim, C., Mashburn, C. A., Martin, J. D., & Engle, R. W. (2019). Reaction time in differential and developmental research: A review and commentary on the problems and alternatives. Psychological Bulletin, 145(5), 508–535. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (2002). Processing emotional facial expressions: The role of anxiety and awareness. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(1), 52–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., & Damjanovic, L. (2006). The eyes are sufficient to produce a threat superiority effect. Emotion, 6(3), 534–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodhew, S. C., & Edwards, M. (2019). Translating experimental paradigms into individual-differences research: Contributions, challenges, and practical recommendations. Consciousness and Cognition, 69, 14–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2019.01.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gur, R. C., Sara, R., Hagendoorn, M., Marom, O., Hughett, P., Macy, L., et al. (2002). A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 115(2), 137–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Sumner, P. (2018). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods, 50(3), 1166–1186. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heeren, A., Mogoase, C., Philippot, P., & McNally, R. J. (2015). Attention bias modification for social anxiety: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 40, 76–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.06.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herry, C., Bach, D. R., Esposito, F., Di Salle, F., Perrig, W. J., Scheffler, K., et al. (2007). Processing of temporal unpredictability in human and animal amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(22), 5958–5966. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5218-06.2007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoviello, B. M., Wu, G., Abend, R., Murrough, J. W., Feder, A., Fruchter, E., et al. (2014). Attention bias variability and symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 27(2), 232–239. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.21899.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kappenman, E. S., Farrens, J. L., Luck, S. J., & Proudfit, G. H. (2014). Behavioral and ERP measures of attentional bias to threat in the dot-probe task: Poor reliability and lack of correlation with anxiety. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1368. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01368.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(10), 1183–1192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., & Van Damme, S. (2005). Time-course of attention for threatening pictures in high and low trait anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(8), 1087–1098. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.08.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kret, M. E., Jaasma, L., Bionda, T., & Wijnen, J. G. (2016). Bonobos (Pan paniscus) show an attentional bias toward conspecifics’ emotions. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences of the United States of America. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1522060113.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kruijt, A.-W., Field, A. P., & Fox, E. (2016). Capturing dynamics of biased attention: New attention bias variability measures the way forward? PLoS ONE, 11(11), e0166600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Retrieved from Gainesville, FL.

  • Lonsdorf, T. B., Juth, P., Rohde, C., Schalling, M., & Ohman, A. (2014). Attention biases and habituation of attention biases are associated with 5-HTTLPR and COMTval158met. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(1), 354–363. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0200-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundqvist, D., Flykt, A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces (KDEF). CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet, 91–630.

  • Macleod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 15–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazidi, M., Vig, K., Ranjbar, S., Ebrahimi, M. R., & Khatibi, A. (2019). Attentional bias and its temporal dynamics among war veterans suffering from chronic pain: Investigating the contribution of post-traumatic stress symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 66, 102115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2019.102115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Some methodological issues in assessing attentional biases for threatening faces in anxiety: A replication study using a modified version of the probe detection task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(6), 595–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2002). Selective orienting of attention to masked threat faces in social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(12), 1403–1414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2006). Time course of attentional bias for fear-relevant pictures in spider-fearful individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1241–1250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., de Bono, J., & Painter, M. (1997). Time course of attentional bias for threat information in non-clinical anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(4), 297–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B., Dixon, C., Fisher, S., Twelftree, H., & McWilliams, A. (2000). Trait anxiety, defensiveness and selective processing of threat: An investigation using two measures of attentional bias. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 1063–1077.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Waters, A. M., & Bradley, B. (2017). Attention bias modification (ABM): Review of effects of multisession abm training on anxiety and threat-related attention in high-anxious individuals. Clinical Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702617696359.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Naim, R., Abend, R., Wald, I., Eldar, S., Levi, O., Fruchter, E., et al. (2015). Threat-related attention bias variability and posttraumatic stress. American Journal of Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14121579.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108(3), 483–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pourtois, G., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2004). Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 14(6), 619–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price, R. B., Kuckertz, J. M., Siegle, G. J., Ladouceur, C. D., Silk, J. S., Ryan, N. D., et al. (2015). Empirical recommendations for improving the stability of the dot-probe task in clinical research. Psychological Assessment, 27(2), 365–376. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000036.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Puls, S., & Rothermund, K. (2017). Attending to emotional expressions: no evidence for automatic capture in the dot-probe task. Cognition and Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1314932.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reutter, M., Hewig, J., Wieser, M. J., & Osinsky, R. (2017). The N2pc component reliably captures attentional bias in social anxiety. Psychophysiology, 54(4), 519–527. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salemink, E., van den Hout, M. A., & Kindt, M. (2007). Selective attention and threat: Quick orienting versus slow disengagement and two versions of the dot probe task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(3), 607–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmukle, S. C. (2005). Unreliability of the dot probe task. European Journal of Personality: Published for the European Association of Personality Psychology, 19(7), 595–605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the State-Trait Axiety Inventory (Self- Evaluation Questionnaire). Palo Alto, CA: Consulating Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staugaard, S. R. (2009). Reliability of two versions of the dot-probe task using photographic faces. Psychology Science Quarterly, 51(3), 339–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swick, D., & Ashley, V. (2017). Enhanced attentional bias variability in post-traumatic stress disorder and its relationship to more general impairments in cognitive control. Science Report, 7(1), 14559. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-15226-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torrence, R. D., Wylie, E., & Carlson, J. M. (2017). The time-course for the capture and hold of visuospatial attention by fearful and happy faces. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016-0247-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Rooijen, R., Ploeger, A., & Kret, M. E. (2017). The dot-probe task to measure emotional attention: A suitable measure in comparative studies? Psychonomic Bulletin Review. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1224-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, M. A., Morrow, K. A., Rizer, W. S., Kangas, K. J., & Carlson, J. M. (2016). Sustained, not habituated, activity in the human amygdala: A pilot fMRI dot-probe study of attentional bias to fearful faces. Cogent Psychology, 3(1), 1259881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, L. K., Britton, J. C., Sequeira, S., Ronkin, E. G., Chen, G., Bar-Haim, Y., et al. (2016). Behavioral and neural stability of attention bias to threat in healthy adolescents. Neuroimage, 136, 84–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.058.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zvielli, A., Bernstein, A., & Koster, E. H. (2014). Dynamics of attentional bias to threat in anxious adults: Bias towards and/or away? PLoS ONE, 9(8), e104025. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104025.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zvielli, A., Bernstein, A., & Koster, E. H. (2015). Temporal dynamics of attentional bias. Clinical Psychological Science, 3(5), 772–788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zvielli, A., Vrijsen, J. N., Koster, E. H., & Bernstein, A. (2016). Attentional bias temporal dynamics in remitted depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(6), 768–776. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R15MH110951. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. We would like to thank Jeremy A. Andrzejewski and Taylor Susa, and all the other students in the Cognitive × Affective Behavior & Integrative Neuroscience (CABIN) Lab at Northern Michigan University for assisting in the collection of this data. In addition, we would like to thank three anonymous reviewers and the editor for their helpful feedback on an earlier draft of this manuscript. In particular, reviewer 3′s suggestion to explore the potential influence of general reaction time variability on the reliability of attention bias indices.

Funding

This study was funded by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R15MH110951.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joshua M. Carlson.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Joshua Carlson declares that he has no conflict of interest. Lin Fang declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Carlson, J.M., Fang, L. The stability and reliability of attentional bias measures in the dot-probe task: Evidence from both traditional mean bias scores and trial-level bias scores. Motiv Emot 44, 657–669 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-020-09834-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-020-09834-6

Keywords

Navigation