Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Assisted versus asserted autonomy satisfaction: Their unique associations with wellbeing, integration of experience, and conflict negotiation

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigate the possibility of two distinct approaches to autonomy satisfaction—one that is contextually “assisted” and one that is individually “asserted”. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses (Pilot Study and Study 1; N = 449) develop and validate the two-factor structure. We then show that asserted and assisted autonomy orientations predict psychological wellbeing through distinct pathways (i.e., highly active/agentic vs. interdependent). In Study 2 (N = 206), we examine the sociodevelopmental antecedents of each type of autonomy satisfaction, revealing that assisted autonomy is associated with having had authoritiative parents, whereas asserted autonomy is associated with having had authoritarian parents. In Study 3 (N = 109) we show that asserted—but not assisted—autonomy predicts the integration of negative life experiences. Finally, in Study 4 (N = 202), we examine the degree to which assisted and asserted autonomy moderate responses to conflict in need-thwarting contexts, showing that assisted autonomy predicts an acquiescent coping style, whereas asserted autonomy predicts an assertive negotiation style.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Across studies, no gender differences were found in asserted autonomy (Masserted for men = 4.22; SD = .98; Masserted for women = 4.29; SD = .91), F < 1. In Study 2 only, women (M = 4.60; SD = .95) showed significantly more assisted autonomy than men (M = 4.27; SD = 1.02), F(1, 204) = 7.249, p < .001. T there were no gender differences in assisted autonomy across the other studies (Mmen = 4.784; SD = .817; Mwomen = 4.741; SD = .931), F < 1.

  2. An alternate one-factor model where both assisted and asserted items were ascribed to the same first-order latent general autonomy factor yielded an undesirable fit, CFI = .727; SRMR = .161; RMSEA = .136, which further justified the proposed two factor structure of autonomy satisfaction. We also tested a standard first order factor structure, with asserted and assisted autonomy specified as separate factors. Not surprisingly, this yielded a fit similar to the second order model, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .07; SRMR = .06.

  3. Although they represent theoretically distinct constructs, the strong correlation between assisted autonomy satisfaction and interpersonal connectedness led us to test a measurement model wherein assisted autonomy items and interpersonal connectedness items were specified to load onto the same “interpersonal” factor. This yielded a poor fit, CF1 = .795; SRMR = .147; RMSEA = .121, suggesting that assisted autonomy and interpersonal connectedness are metrically (as well as conceptually) different.

  4. Although this manipulation was hypothetical in nature, credibility of the scenarios in supporting or frustrating psychological needs and wellbeing was assessed by measuring state negative mood using 9 items adapted from the PANAS (i.e., Right now I feel: distressed, upset, guilty, scared, hostile, irritable, ashamed, nervous, jittery). Negative mood was assessed both before and after reading the scenario. Changes in negative mood were significantly different across conditions, F(1, 198) = 6.046, p = .015, η 2p  = .031, such that those exposed to the need thwarting manager showed an increase in negative mood (M = .360; SD = .277), whereas those exposed to the supportive manager showed a decrease in negative mood (M = −.057; SD = .234). Changes in negative mood were not related to assisted (r = .03, p = .674) or asserted autonomy (r = .01, p = .883). Also, changes in negative mood were not associated with the outcomes of accommodation (r = −.09 ns) or negotiation (r = −.04 ns). However, an increase in negative mood was associated with rumination (r = .28, p < .001).

References

  • Adie, J. W., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2012). Perceived coach-autonomy support, basic need satisfaction and the well- and ill-being of elite youth soccer players: A longitudinal investigation. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13, 51–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumrind, D. (1971). Current patterns of parental authority. Developmental Psychology, 4(1p2), 1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonanno, G. A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human resilience: Have we underestimated the human capacity to thrive after extremely aversive events? American Psychologist, 59(1), 20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bostic, T. J., Rubio, D. M., & Hood, M. (2000). A validation of the subjective vitality scale using structural equation modeling. Social Indicators Research, 52, 313–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buri, J. R. (1991). Parental authority questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57(1), 110–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chatzisarantis, N. L., Hagger, M. S., Kamarova, S., & Kawabata, M. (2012). When effects of universal psychological needs on health behaviour extend to a large proportion of individuals. British Journal of Health Psychology, 4, 785–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Beyers, W., Boone, L., Deci, E. L., Van der Kaap-Deeder, J., et al. (2015). Basic psychological need satisfaction, need frustration, and need strength across four cultures. Motivation and Emotion, 39(2), 216–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 233–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cumming, G. (2009). Inference by eye: Reading the overlap of independent confidence intervals. Statistics in Medicine, 28(2), 205–220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The general causality orientations scale: Self-determination in personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 19(2), 109–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The ‘what’ and ‘why’ of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2013). The importance of autonomy for development and well-being. In B. W. Sokol, F. M. E. Grouzet, & U. Muller (Eds.), Self-regulation and autonomy: Social and developmental dimensions of human conduct (pp. 19–46). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagné, M., Leone, D. R., Usunov, J., & Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). Need satisfaction, motivation, and well-being in the work organizations of a former Eastern Bloc country. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 930–942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finkel, E. J., & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Self-control and accommodation in close relationships: An interdependence analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(2), 263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flowers, P., & Buston, K. (2001). “I was terrified of being different”: Exploring gay men’s accounts of growing-up in a heterosexist society. Journal of Adolescence, 24(1), 51–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gagné, M. (2003). The role of autonomy support and autonomy orientation in prosocial behavior engagement. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 199–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodgins, H. S., & Knee, C. R. (2002). The integrating self and conscious experience. In E. L. Deci, & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 87–100). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kashdan, T. B., Gallagher, M. W., Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Breen, W. E., Terhar, D., et al. (2009). The Curiosity and Exploaration Inventory-II: Development, factor, structure, and psychometrics. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 987–998.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Koestner, R., & Losier, G. F. (1996). Distinguishing reactive versus reflective autonomy. Journal of Personality, 64(2), 465–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legate, N., Ryan, R. M., & Weinstein, N. (2012). Is coming out always a “good thing”? Exploring the relations of autonomy support, outness, and wellness for lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 145–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legault, L., Weinstein, N., Mitchell, J., Inzlicht, M., Pyke, K., & Upal, A. (2016). Owning up to negative ingroup traits: How personal autonomy promotes the integration of group identity. Journal of Personality. doi:10.1111/jopy.12277.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lu, L., & Gilmour, R. (2007). Developing a new measure of independent and interdependent views of the self. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 249–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, M., & Harris, J. (1997a). The desire for unique consumer products: A new individual differences scale. Psychology and Marketing, 14, 601–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, M., & Harris, J. (1997b). Individual differences in the pursuit of self-uniqueness through consumption. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27, 1861–1883.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masten, A. S., Best, K. M., & Garmezy, N. (1990). Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 2(04), 425–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radel, R., Pelletier, L., Baxter, D., Fournier, M., & Sarrazin, P. (2014). The paradoxical effect of controlling context on intrinsic motivation in another activity. Learning and Instruction, 29, 95–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radel, R., Pelletier, L., & Sarrazin, P. (2013). Restoration processes after need thwarting: When autonomy depends on competence. Motivation and Emotion, 37(2), 234–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radel, R., Pelletier, L. G., Sarrazin, P., & Milyavskaya, M. (2011). Restoration process of the need for autonomy: The early alarm stage. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 919–934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reeve, J. (2013). How students create motivationally supportive learning environments for themselves: The concept of agentic engagement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(3), 579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robitschek, C. (1999). Further validation of the Personal Growth Initiative Scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 31(4), 197–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robitschek, C., & Keyes, C. L. (2009). Keyes’s model of mental health with personal growth initiative as a parsimonious predictor. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56(2), 321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, C. R. (1963). The actualizing tendency in relation to “motives” and to consciousness. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 1–24). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosario, M., Schrimshaw, E. W., Hunter, J., & Braun, L. (2006). Sexual identity development among lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths: Consistency and change over time. Journal of Sex Research, 43(1), 46–58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, E. A., Markland, D., & Parfitt, G. (2001). The development and initial validation of the Exercise Causality Orientations Scale. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19(6), 445–462.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rusbult, C. E., Yovetich, N. A., & Verette, J. (1996). An interdependence analysis of accommodation processes. In G. J. O. Fletcher & J. Fitness (Eds.), Knowledge structures in close relationships: A social psychological approach (pp. 63–90). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rusbult, C. E., & Zembrodt, I. M. (1983). Responses to dissatisfaction in romantic involvements: A multidimensional scaling analysis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19(3), 274–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes. Journal of Personality, 63, 397–427.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2003). On assimilating identities to the self: A self-determination theory perspective on internalization and integrity within cultures. In M. R. Leary, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook on self & identity (pp. 253–274). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., Grolnick, W. S., & La Guardia, J. G. (2006). The significance of autonomy and autonomy support in psychological development and psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Theory and method (Vol. 1, pp. 795–849). New Jersey: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. Journal of Personality, 65(3), 529–565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need-satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 482–497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Gunz, A. (2009). Psychological needs as basic motives, not just experiential requirements. Journal of Personality, 77(5), 1467–1492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Niemiec, C. P. (2006). It’s not just the amount that counts: Balanced need satisfaction also affects well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 331–341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2006). Exploring the psychology of interest. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, C. R., & Fromkin, H. L. (1980). Uniqueness: The human pursuit of difference. New York: Plenum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Soenens, B., & Beyers, W. (2012). The cross-cultural significance of control and autonomy in parent–adolescent relationships. Journal of Adolescence, 35, 243–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-McCoy, S., & Sugarman, D. B. (1996). The Revised Conflict Tactics Scale (CTS2): Development and preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17, 283–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tremblay, M. A., Blanchard, C. M., Taylor, S., Pelletier, L. G., & Villeneuve, M. (2009). Work Extrinsic and Intrinsic Motivation Scale: Its value for organizational psychology research. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 41(4), 213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Petegem, S., Vansteenkiste, M., Soenens, B., Beyers, W., & Aelterman, N. (2014). Examining the longitudinal association between oppositional defiance and autonomy in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 51, 67–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vansteenkiste, M., & Ryan, R. M. (2013). On psychological growth and vulnerability: Basic psychological need satisfaction and need frustration as a unifying principle. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 23, 263–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2011). Motivational determinants of integrating positive and negative past identities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 527–544.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N., Przybylski, A. K., & Ryan, R. M. (2012a). The index of autonomous functioning: Development of a scale of human autonomy. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(4), 397–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N., Ryan, W. S., DeHaan, C. R., Przybylski, A. K., Legate, N., & Ryan, R. M. (2012b). Parental autonomy support and discrepancies between implicit and explicit sexual identities: Dynamics of self-acceptance and defense. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 815–832.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lisa Legault.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

None of the authors have any conflict of interest involving any aspect of this research.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Appendix: Need-thwarting versus need-supportive interpersonal scenario (Study 4)

Appendix: Need-thwarting versus need-supportive interpersonal scenario (Study 4)

Imagine your worst, most controlling boss

Imagine that you are working for a mid-size company, as a data entry clerk. You are required to be in the office from 8:00a.m. until 4:30p.m. every day, recording and processing sales data. Your boss is rude to his employees; he constantly tells everyone what to do (and how to do it), and criticizes their abilities. But, you feel that he is especially controlling with you. He requires you to complete all the jobs that no one else wants to do, without giving you any say in the matter. He barks orders and demands at you multiple times a day. When you are unable to complete tasks due to time constraints or interruptions, he does not accept your excuses and instead says that you are inadequate. To make matters worse, he often excludes you from team meetings and organized events, and plays favorites with some of your other colleagues. Finally, he frequently belittles you in front of others—pointing out your mistakes and character flaws.

Try to visualize this boss, and his behaviors. Take a few minutes to create this scenario in your mind.

Imagine your best, most supportive boss

Imagine that you are working for a mid-size company, as a data entry clerk. You work approximately 8 h per day—mostly recording and processing sales data. Your boss understands that the work is not always exciting, and so he tries to support you by giving you flexible hours, and letting you choose the projects and tasks you want to work on. Your boss also helps you through the less interesting aspects of your job by explaining why the work is meaningful and helpful, and gives you training and opportunity to develop your analytic skills and other professional abilities. He also tries to make work fun by tailoring tasks to your personal preferences and goals. He provides you with constructive feedback, and believes in your potential to be promoted. Your boss generally likes you as a person and wants to see you succeed.

Try to visualize this boss, and his behaviors. Take a few minutes to create this scenario in your mind.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Legault, L., Ray, K., Hudgins, A. et al. Assisted versus asserted autonomy satisfaction: Their unique associations with wellbeing, integration of experience, and conflict negotiation. Motiv Emot 41, 1–21 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9593-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9593-3

Keywords

Navigation