Skip to main content
Log in

The implicit need for power predicts recognition speed for dynamic changes in facial expressions of emotion

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Facial expressions of emotion (FEEs) have been portrayed as potent (dis-) incentives for power-motivated perceivers, because they signal the strength of a sender’s dominance (Stanton et al. in Implicit motives. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 245–278, 2010). Here, we tested the hypothesis that individuals with a high implicit power motive (nPower), who have a disposition to seek (emotional) impact on others, would be faster at recognizing FEEs than individuals low in nPower. In a task employing videos of morphed FEEs, which are gradually changing from neutral to either anger, joy or surprise, higher nPower predicted faster recognition of the displayed emotion as well as a tendency to misidentify joy as anger. Our findings suggest that one way through which people high in nPower are socially influential is their enhanced sensitivity to emotional signals in their social environment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adelmann, P. K., & Zajonc, R. B. (1989). Facial efference and the experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 40(1), 249–280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borsboom, D., Mellenbergh, G. J., & van Heerden, J. (2004). The concept of validity. Psychological Review, 111, 1061–1071.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Hager, J. C. (2002). Facial action coding system. The manual on CD ROM. Salt Lake City, UT: Research Nexus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & O’Sullivan, M. (1991). Who can catch a liar? American Psychologist, 46(9), 913–920.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, E. M., & Carver, R. A. (2000). Achievement and power motives, performance feedback, and creativity. Journal of Research in Personality, 34(4), 380–396. doi:10.1006/jrpe.2000.2289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, E. M., Wick, D. P., & Hartsen, K. M. (2006). The power motive and affective response to assertiveness. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(5), 598–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J., Andrzejewski, S., & Yopchick, J. (2009). Psychosocial correlates of interpersonal sensitivity: A meta-analysis. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33(3), 149–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hareli, S., Shomrat, N., & Hess, U. (2009). Emotional versus neutral expressions and perceptions of social dominance and submissiveness. Emotion, 9(3), 378–384.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., Blairy, S., & Kleck, R. E. (2000). The influence of facial emotion displays, gender, and ethnicity on judgments of dominance and affiliation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24, 265–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110(2), 265–284.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Knutson, B. (1996). Facial expressions of emotion influence interpersonal trait inferences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20(3), 165–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köllner, M. G., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2014). Meta-analytic evidence of low convergence between implicit and explicit measures of the needs for achievement, affiliation, and power. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 826.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human motivation. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., & Boyatzis, R. E. (1982). Leadership motive pattern and long-term success in management. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 737–743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, D. C., & Burnham, D. H. (1976). Power is the great motivator. Harvard Business Review, 54, 100–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • N’Diaye, K., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2009). Self-relevance processing in the human amygdala: Gaze direction, facial expression, and emotion intensity. Emotion, 9(6), 798–806.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rawolle, M., Schultheiss, M., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2013). Relationships between implicit motives, self-attributed motives, and personal goal commitments. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 923.

  • Rösch, A. G., & Schultheiss, O. C. (n.d.). The standardized and motivated facial expression of emotion (SMoFEE) stimulus set: I. Evaluation of expressive and perceptual validity (in preparation).

  • Schultheiss, O. C. (2008). Implicit motives. In O. John, R. Robins, & L. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 603–633). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2002). Inhibited power motivation and persuasive communication: A lens model analysis. Journal of Personality, 70(4), 553–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., & Hale, J. A. (2007). Implicit motives modulate attentional orienting to facial expressions of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 31(1), 13–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., & Pang, J. S. (2007). Measuring implicit motives. In R. W. Robins, C. Fraley, & R. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 322–344). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., Pang, J. S., Torges, C. M., Wirth, M. M., & Treynor, W. (2005). Perceived facial expressions of emotion as motivational incentives: Evidence from a differential implicit learning paradigm. Emotion, 5(1), 41–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., Wirth, M. M., & Stanton, S. J. (2004). Effects of affiliation and power motivation arousal on salivary progesterone and testosterone. Hormones and Behavior, 46(5), 592–599.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., Wirth, M. M., Waugh, C. E., Stanton, S. J., Meier, E. A., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2008). Exploring the motivational brain: effects of implicit power motivation on brain activation in response to facial expressions of emotion. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(4), 333–343.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schultheiss, O. C., Yankova, D., Dirlikov, B., & Schad, D. J. (2009). Are implicit and explicit motive measures statistically independent? A fair and balanced test using the picture story exercise and a cue- and response-matched questionnaire measure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(1), 72–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112(2), 140–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanton, S. J., Hall, J. L., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2010). Properties of motive-specific incentives. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 245–278). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, J., Liu, L., & Yan, J. H. (2014). Implicit power motive effects on the ERP processing of emotional intensity in anger faces. Journal of Research in Personality, 50, 90–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, J., Liu, L., & Zheng, Y. (2011). Effects of implicit power motive on the processing of anger faces: An event-related potential study. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(5), 441–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G. (1988). The power motive in women—And men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(3), 510–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G. (1994). Manual for scoring motive imagery in running text (4th ed.). Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor: Unpublished manuscript.

  • Winter, D. G. (2010). Political and historical consequences of implicit motives. In Implicit motives (pp. 407–432).

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft [grant SCHU 1210/3-1] awarded to Oliver C. Schultheiss and Andreas G. Rösch and a McClelland Fellowship of the McClelland Center for Research and Innovation (Hay Group) awarded to Andreas G. Rösch.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter W. Donhauser.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Donhauser, P.W., Rösch, A.G. & Schultheiss, O.C. The implicit need for power predicts recognition speed for dynamic changes in facial expressions of emotion. Motiv Emot 39, 714–721 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9484-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9484-z

Keywords

Navigation