Skip to main content
Log in

Rose-colored glasses gone too far? Mania symptoms predict biased emotion experience and perception in couples

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study investigated how symptoms of mania—associated with heightened and persistent positive emotion—influence emotion experience and perception during distressing social interactions, whereby the experience of heightened positive emotion may not be socially adaptive. Specifically, mania symptoms were assessed via a validated self-report measure, and used to predict emotion experience and perception during a naturalistic conversation between romantic couples about a time of distress and suffering (N = 68 dyads). Results indicated that mania symptoms were associated with increased positive and decreased negative emotion experience and perception between couples, as well as decreased empathic accuracy for partners’ negative but not positive emotions. These findings suggest that mania symptoms may be associated with “rose-colored” glasses characterized by a positively biased emotional experience and outward perception even during perhaps the most intimate and distressing social situations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Because individual emotion findings were generally consistent with the results from the composites, and internal consistency was high within the scale, we used composites rather than individual emotion items for statistical parsimony.

  2. Follow-up analyses including role (speaker vs. listener) in the models revealed that role did not moderate effects of mania symptoms (ASRM scores) on positive or negative emotion experience or perception (ps > .60). For the empathic accuracy analyses, role also did not moderate the directional bias of ASRM (ps > .40) or the Truth Force (ps > .20), and the three-way interaction between ASRM scores, truth force, and role was not significant (ps > .30) for positive or negative emotion experience or perception composites.

  3. For positive emotion, gender did not moderate the directional bias or truth force (ps > .80), but did moderate the bias force (b = −0.09, t(186.00) = −2.49, p < .05), such that men demonstrated a weaker bias force compared to women; that is, women assumed more similarity than did men. In the same model, partner ASRM scores did not moderate the directional bias, truth force, or bias force (ps > .10). Consistent with what is reported in the main text, in this model, directional bias was negative and significant, (b = −0.13, t(99.82) = −2.89, p < .01), the truth force was positive and significant (b = 0.14, t(239.54) = 2.96, p < .01), and the bias force was positive and significant (b = 0.81, t(239.43) = 22.12, p < .001). Perceiver ASRM of the perceiver did not moderate directional bias, the truth force, or the bias force (ps > .20).

  4. For negative emotion, gender did not moderate the directional bias (p > .10) but did moderate the truth force (b = −0.12, t(208.91) = −2.31, p < .05), such that men were less accurate at perceiving negative emotion in their partners overall. Gender did not moderate the bias force (p > .90), but partner ASRM scores did moderate the directional bias (b = 0.03, t(250.39) = 3.17, p < .01), such that individuals whose partners scored higher in ASRM were more likely to overestimate their partners’ negative emotions. Partner ASRM scores did not moderate the truth force or bias force (ps > .10). In this model, consistent with what is reported in the main text, directional bias was positive and significant, (b = 0.16, t(124.08) = 4.12, p < .001), the truth force was positive and significant (b = 0.42, t(247.85) = 8.41, p < .001), and the bias force was positive and significant (b = 0.44, t(240) = 8.83, p < .01). Perceivers with higher ASRM scores overestimated their partners’ negative emotion less than those with lower ASRM scores (b = −0.03, t(251.11) = −3.07, p < .01). Perceivers with higher ASRM scores were less accurate in their perceptions of their partners’ negative emotions than those with lower scores (b = −0.03, t(250.58) = −2.89, p < .01), and individuals with higher ASRM scores assumed similarity to a weaker extent than did those with lower ASRM scores (b = −0.03, t(250.43) = −3.00, p < .01).

References

  • Altman, E., Hedeker, D., Peterson, J. L., & Davis, J. M. (1997). The altman self-rating mania scale. Biological Psychiatry, 42, 948–955.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Altman, E., Hedeker, D., Peterson, J. L., & Davis, J. M. (2001). A comparative evaluation of three self-rating scales for acute mania. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 468–471.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

  • Butler, E. A., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2006). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, emotion, and emotion regulation during social interaction. Psychophysiology, 43, 612–622.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dilsaver, S. C. (2011). An estimate of the minimum economic burden of bipolar I and II disorders in the United States: 2009. Journal of Affective Disorders, 129, 79–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epley, N., Morewedge, C. K., & Keysar, B. (2004). Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 760–768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, A. H., & van Kleef, G. A. (2010). Where have all the people gone? A plea for including social interaction in emotion research. Emotion Review, 2, 208–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, G. J., & Kerr, P. S. (2010). Through the eyes of love: Reality and illusion in intimate relationships. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 627–658.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: An evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 351–374.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, J. (2011). Can feeling too good be bad? Positive emotion persistence (PEP) in bipolar disorder. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 217–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, J., Harvey, A. G., & Purcell, A. L. (2011a). What goes up can come down? A preliminary investigation of emotion reactivity and emotion recovery in bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 133, 457–466.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, J., Johnson, S. L., Oveis, C., & Keltner, D. (2008). Risk for mania and positive emotional responding: Too much of a good thing? Emotion, 8, 23–33.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, J., Mauss, I. B., & Tamir, M. (2011b). A dark side of happiness? How, when, and why happiness is not always good. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 222–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, J. A., & Schmid Mast, M. (2008). Are women always more interpersonally sensitive than men? Impact of goals and content domain. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 144–155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ickes, W., Gesn, P. R., & Graham, T. (2005). Gender differences in empathic accuracy: Differential ability or differential motivation? Personal Relationships, 7, 95–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Impett, E. A., Gordon, A. M., Kogan, A., Oveis, C., Gable, S. L., & Keltner, D. (2010). Moving toward more perfect unions: Daily and long-term consequences of approach and avoidance goals in romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 948–963.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. L., Gruber, J., & Eisner, L. (2007). Emotion in bipolar disorder. In J. Rotternberg & S. L. Johnson (Eds.), Emotion and psychopathology: Bridging affective and clinical science. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association (APA) Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Cook, W. L. (2006). Analysis of dyadic data. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, E., Jung, Y.-C., Ku, J., Kim, J.-J., Lee, H., Kim, S. Y., et al. (2009). Reduced activation in the mirror neuron system during a virtual social cognition task in euthymic bipolar disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 197, 412–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lembke, A., & Ketter, T. A. (2002). Impaired recognition of facial emotion in mania. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 302–304.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malhi, G. S., Lagopoulos, J., Das, P., Moss, K., Berk, M., & Coulston, C. M. (2008). A functional MRI study of theory of mind in euthymic bipolar disorder patients. Bipolar Disorders, 10, 943–956.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nickerson, R. S. (1999). How we know—and sometimes misjudge—What others know: Imputing one’s own knowledge to others. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 737–759.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romans, S. I., & McPherson, H. M. (2002). The social networks of bipolar affective disorder patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 25, 221–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevisani, D., Johnson, S. L., & Carver, C. S. (2008). Positive mood induction and facial affect recognition among students at risk for mania. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 639–650.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • West, T. V., & Kenny, D. A. (2011). The truth and bias model of judgment. Psychological Review, 118, 357–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelm, P., & Perrez, M. (2004). How is my partner feeling in different daily-life settings? Accuracy of spouses’ judgments about their partner’s feelings at work and at home. Social Indicators Research, 67, 183–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaki, J., Bolger, N., & Ochsner, K. (2008). It takes two: The interpersonal nature of empathic accuracy. Psychological Science, 19, 399–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to June Gruber.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dutra, S.J., West, T.V., Impett, E.A. et al. Rose-colored glasses gone too far? Mania symptoms predict biased emotion experience and perception in couples. Motiv Emot 38, 157–165 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9363-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9363-4

Keywords

Navigation