Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Goal conflict, goal striving, and psychological well-being

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Although goal conflict is an important part of classic and contemporary theories of motivation, the correlates of goal conflict are not well understood. We identify and distinguish conflicting and facilitating goals, and assess relations with goal attainment and psychological well-being in a short-term, prospective study design. Results from multilevel models demonstrated that individuals with greater conflict were less successful in attaining their goals, but the goals they failed attain were not necessarily the ones in conflict. People who experienced goal conflict tended to be ruminative and hesitant, and reported greater levels of negative affect and increases in depression, anxiety, and psychosomatization. People who experienced goal facilitation reported greater levels of positive affect, life satisfaction, and successful goal attainment. This study identifies several implications of holding conflicting and facilitating goals, but also points to a theoretical inconsistency pertaining to goal conflict. Namely, conflicting goals may not be inherently less attainable than nonconflicting goals. We argue that distinguishing between goal- and person-level factors is essential for understanding goal striving.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. These and other significance tests of within-person correlations are based on treating the N = 180 r-to-z transformed correlations as scores and testing the null hypothesis that the values were sampled from a distribution with a mean of 0.00. Reported mean values in the text have been transformed back to r.

  2. A panel of three undergraduate research assistants classified each goal into a hierarchical goal taxonomy structured around eight broad content domains, including academic/occupational, social relationships, financial concerns, health and fitness, organization, affect control, independence, and moral/religious. Also included is an “other” category for meta-goals and acculturation/language goals. A participant’s goal was content-coded when two of the three raters agreed on its assignment. Raters agreed on 1,187 of 1,356 goals (87.5 % agreement). For the remaining 169 goals, two independent raters classified each goal, and they agreed on 140 of them bringing the consensus rate to 97.9 %. The remaining 29 goals were classified via a roundtable discussion by the authors and research assistants.

  3. We computed a state orientation composite by averaging over the preoccupation and hesitation scale items to include in the multilevel modeling analyses. Separate analyses were run for the preoccupation and hesitation facets. The results were consistent with those for the state orientation composite, although they were somewhat smaller in magnitude. We present the results for the composite variable for sake of parsimony.

  4. Freud’s simple and insightful sentiment toward mental health was the ability “to love and to work” (Erikson 1963, pp. 264–265). It is interesting to see here that the majority of goal conflicts reported by young adults involve these two principal domains of life, and that people struggling with these tasks reported greater levels of depression, anxiety, and psychosomatization.

References

  • Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psychology: Structure, process, and content. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 338–375. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.120.3.338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cantor, N., Acker, M., & Cook-Flannagan, C. (1992). Conflict and preoccupation in the intimacy life task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 644–655. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.63.4.644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.97.1.19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Derogatis, L. R. (2000). Brief symptom inventory 18. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems, Pearson, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 276–302. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.125.2.276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emmons, R. A., & King, L. A. (1988). Conflict among personal strivings. Immediate and long- term implications for psychological and physical well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1040–1048. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Emmons, R. A., King, L. A., & Sheldon, K. (1993). Goal conflict and the self-regulation of action. In D. M. Wegner & J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control (pp. 528–551). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1982). Conflict and stress. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Handbook of stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects (pp. 49–68). New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. H. (1963). Child and society. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M. E., & Ford, D. H. (1987). Humans as self-constructing living systems: Putting the framework to work. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associated, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A., & McNaughton, N. (2000). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grzywacz, J. G., & Marks, N. F. (2000). Reconceptualizing the work-family interface: An ecological perspective on the correlates of positive and negative spillover between work and family. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 111–126. doi:10.1037//1076-8998.5.1.111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, R. T., & Ozer, D. J. (1997). Emotional stability and goal-related stress. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 371–379. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00208-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kehr, H. M. (2003). Goal conflicts, attainment of new goals, and well-being among managers. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 8, 195–208. doi:10.1037/1076-8998.8.3.195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, R. E., Mansell, W., & Wood, A. M. (2011). Goal conflict and ambivalence interact to predict depression. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 531–534. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.11.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • King, L. A., Richards, J. H., & Stemmerich, E. (1998). Daily goals, life goals, and worst fears: Means, ends, and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 66, 713–744. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koestner, R., Lekes, N., Powers, T. A., & Chicoine, E. (2002). Attaining personal goals: Self-concordance plus implementation intentions equals success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 231–244. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.83.1.231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kopetz, C., Fishbach, A., Faber, T., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2011). The multifinality constraints effects: How goal multiplicity narrows the means set to a focal end. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110, 810–826. doi:10.1037/a0022980.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1994a). A theory of action and state orientation. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmänn (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 9–46). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1994b). Action versus state orientation: Psychometric properties of the Action Control Scale (ACS-90). In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmänn (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action versus state orientation (pp. 47–59). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality: Selected papers (D. E. Adams & K. E. Zener, Trans). New York: McGraw-Hill.

  • Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705–717. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.57.9.705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, I., & Little, B. R. (1998). Personal projects, happiness, and meaning: On doing well and being yourself. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 494–512. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.74.2.494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McReynolds, P. (1990). The nature and logic of intrapsychic conflicts. In C. D. Spielberger, I. G. Sarason, J. Strelau, & J. M. T. Brebner (Eds.), Stress and anxiety (Vol. 3, pp. 73–83). New York: Hemisphere.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michalak, J., & Schulte, D. (2002). Zielkonflikte und Therapiemotivation [Goal conflicts and therapy motivation]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 31, 213–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. (1944). Experimental studies of conflict. In J. Mc. V. Hunt (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders (pp. 431–465). New York: Ronald Press Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., & Congdon, T. (2004). HLM 6 for windows [Computer software]. Lincoln, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riediger, M. (2007). Interference and facilitation among personal goals: Age differences and associations with well-being and behavior. In B. R. Little, K. Salmela-Aro, & S. D. Phillips (Eds.), Personal project pursuit: Goals, action, and human flourishing (pp. 119–143). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riediger, M., & Freund, A. M. (2004). Interference and facilitation among personal goals: Differential associations with subjective well-being and persistent goal pursuit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1511–1523. doi:10.1177/0146167204271184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riediger, M., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). Managing life through personal goals: Intergoal facilitation and intensity of goal pursuit in younger and older adulthood. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 60, P84–P91. doi:10.1093/geronb/60.2.P84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segerstrom, S. C., & Solberg Nes, L. (2006). When goals conflict but people prosper: The case of dispositional optimism. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 675–693. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M. (1995). Creativity and goal conflict. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 299–306. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj0803_9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Houser-Marko, L. (2001). Self-concordance, goal-attainment, and the pursuit of happiness: Can there be an upward spiral? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 152–165. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.80.1.152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1995). Coherence and congruence: Two aspects of personality integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 531–543. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.68.3.531.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilensky, R. (1983). Planning and understanding: A computational approach to human reasoning. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Michael J. Boudreaux would like to thank Dr. Kate Sweeny for her helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael J. Boudreaux.

Additional information

A portion of the data reported in this study was described in a poster session at the 2012 annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Boudreaux, M.J., Ozer, D.J. Goal conflict, goal striving, and psychological well-being. Motiv Emot 37, 433–443 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9333-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-012-9333-2

Keywords

Navigation