Skip to main content
Log in

Stress reduces attention to irrelevant information: Evidence from the Stroop task

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Stroop interference can be reduced by stress, and this has been taken as evidence that stress decreases the attention paid to irrelevant information, a theory known as ‘Easterbrook’s hypothesis’. This contradicts more recent theories, which state that attentional control deteriorates in stress. Fifty-five participants undertook a Stroop task under high stress (loud white noise) and low stress conditions. Attention to the irrelevant word information was assessed by manipulating the proportion of congruent trials (e.g. the word RED in the colour red); it is known that Stroop interference increases when many such trials are presented. This effect was reduced when participants were stressed, which is evidence that stress does indeed reduce attention to irrelevant information. This pattern of results was not present in participants with low working memory spans, presumably because these participants had less attentional control. These findings highlight an important weakness in contemporary theories of cognition in stress.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The Stroop data were re-analysed with the upper and lower tertiles as the WM groups. The same pattern of results were found as reported in Fig. 1. Furthermore, when Proportion-Congruent effects were calculated by subtracting 0% condition interference scores from 50% condition interference scores, it was found that Proportion-Congruent effects in the 84 dBC condition tended to correlate negatively with Ospan score (r(53) = −0.224, p = 0.099); Proportion-Congruent effects in the 65 dBC condition did not correlate with Ospan scores (r(53) = 0.123, p = 0.26). These findings support our conclusions from our median split analyses.

  2. Although this tendency did not approach significance for RT interference, t(52) = 0.70, p = 0.49, or accuracy interference, t(52) = 1.30, p = 0.20.

References

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556–559.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blais, C., Robidoux, S., Risko, E. F., & Besner, D. (2007). Item-specific adaptation and the conflict-monitoring hypothesis: A computational model. Psychological Review, 114, 1076–1086.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Callaway, E. (1959). The influence of amobarbital (amylobarbitone) and methamphetamine on the focus of attention. Journal of Mental Science, 105, 382–392.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chajut, E., & Algom, D. (2003). Selective attention improves under stress: Implications for theories of social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 231–248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Fockert, J. W., Rees, G., Frith, C. D., & Lavie, N. (2001). The role of working memory in visual selective attention. Science, 291, 1803–1806.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frankish, C. (2002). Lexicus (Version 3.0.1): University of Bristol.

  • Geen, R. G., & McCown, E. J. (1984). Effects of noise and attack on aggression and physiological arousal. Motivation and Emotion, 8, 231–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huguet, P., Dumas, F., & Monteil, J. M. (2004). Competing for a desired reward in the Stroop task: When attentional control is unconscious but effective versus conscious but ineffective. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 153–167.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R. A., Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2007). Variation in working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control. In A. R. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. N. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 21–48). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenemans, J. L., Wieleman, J. S. T., Zeegers, M., & Verbaten, M. N. (1999). Caffeine and Stroop interference. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behaviour, 63, 589–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N., Hirst, A., de Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 339–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay, D. S., & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Stroop process dissociations: The relationship between facilitation and interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 219–234.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & Mackintosh, B. (1998). A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 539–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McFall, S. R., Jamieson, J. P., & Harkins, S. G. (2009). Testing the mere effort account of the evaluation-performance relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 135–154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melara, R. D., & Algom, D. (2003). Driven by information: A tectonic theory of Stroop effects. Psychological Review, 110, 422–471.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Malley, J. J., & Gallas, J. (1977). Noise and attention span. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 919–922.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Malley, J. J., & Poplawsky, A. (1971). Noise-induced arousal and breadth of attention. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 33, 887–890.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pallak, M. S., Pittman, T. S., Heller, J. F., & Munson, P. (1975). The effect of arousal on Stroop color-word task performance. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 6, 248–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 653–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tuholski, S., & Engle, R. W. (2002). OSPAN E-prime version. Retrieved March 17, 2005, from http://psychology.gatech.edu/renglelab.

  • Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 631–650.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weltman, G., Smith, J. E., & Ergstrom, G. H. (1971). Perceptual narrowing during simulated pressure-chamber exposure. Human Factors, 13, 99–107.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rob Booth.

Additional information

This research was carried out while Rob Booth was a doctoral student at the University of Kent, supervised by Dinkar Sharma.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Booth, R., Sharma, D. Stress reduces attention to irrelevant information: Evidence from the Stroop task. Motiv Emot 33, 412–418 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-009-9141-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-009-9141-5

Keywords

Navigation