Skip to main content
Log in

Can Item-Level Error Correlations Correct for Projection Bias in Perceived Peer Deviance Measures? A Research Note

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Quantitative Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Objectives

Research indicates respondents overestimate the similarity between their own deviance and that of their peers. Extending Rebellon and Modecki’s (J Quant Criminol 30:163–186, 2014) study, we examine if item-level error correlations in structural models reduce bias for non-peer-based, theoretically derived covariates such as self-control. Our specific interest lies in investigating the theoretical implications and practical value of using the correlated error technique in ‘everyday’ structural equation modeling.

Methods

Using dyadic data and multiple constructs of deviance, we present three sets of structural equation analyses. The first assesses the relationship between peer behavior and deviance via perceptual measures. The second uses identical constructs, but estimates item-level error correlations between perceptual and deviance items. The third replaces perceptions of peer deviance with items measuring peers’ self-reported behavior.

Results

Self-control and demographic variables have equivalent effects in perceptually-based correlated error models and models controlling peer self-reported deviance. However, latent variable adjustments to perceptions of peer behavior fail to bring perceived peer deviance coefficients into line with corresponding coefficients from models using peer self-reports, indicating that perceptions and peer self-reports are distinct constructs.

Conclusion

Researchers cannot use item-level error-correlations to model peer effects without collecting data from peers. They may, however, use these correlations to control for peer effects even when peer self-reports are not available. Because we find strong effects of self-control while maintaining social learning theory’s emphasis on perceptions, we argue that the technique is a form of theoretical reconciliation and recommend criminologists adopt the use of correlated errors in all social influence-based structural models.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This means the first set of models is nested within the second set of models.

  2. The violence construct has only three items. Since a CFA with three items necessarily fits the data perfectly, the factor structure of the self-reported and perceptual violence measures was determined by principle component analyses (PCAs). Dimensionality was diagnosed based on proportional eigen decomposition and the elbow rule on scree plots. Consistently, the PCAs demonstrated that the three self-reported and perceptual violence items were in fact measuring one distinct construct, respectively.

  3. We do not include age as a covariate due to its limited variability in our sample of undergraduate college students (μ = 19.3 years old, SD = 1.4 years).

  4. Minor amounts of missing data (average item missingness was less than 1 %) were imputed with the full-information maximum likelihood technique.

  5. Because of disagreement in how self-control should be measured (e.g., Evans et al. 1997), we re-estimated all analyses with a behavioral measure of self-control called the ‘Retrospective Behavioral Self-Control Scale-reduced’ (RBS-r; see Ward et al. 2010). The results with the behavioral measure were identical to those reported with the attitudinal Grasmick et al. (1993) scale.

References

  • Agnew R (1991) The interactive effect of peer variables on delinquency. Criminology 29:47–72

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Akers RL (2009) Social learning and social structure: a general theory of crime and deviance. Transactional Publishers, Brunswick

    Google Scholar 

  • Boman JH, Gibson C (2011) Does the measurement of peer deviance change the relationship between self-control and deviant behavior? An analysis of friendship pairs. J Crim Justice 39:521–530

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boman JH, Stogner JM, Krohn MD, Gibson CL, Stogner JM (2012) Investigating friendship quality: an exploration of self-control and social control theories’ friendship hypotheses. J Youth Adolesc 41:1526–1540

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell L, Kashy DA (2002) Estimating actor, partner and interaction effects for dyadic data using PROC MIXED and HLM: a user-friendly guide. Pers Relatsh 9:327–342

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costello B (1997) On the logical adequacy of cultural deviance theories. Theor Criminol 1:403–428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott DS, Huizinga D, Ageton SS (1985) Explaining delinquency and drug use. Sage, Beverly Hills

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans TD, Cullen FT, Burton VS Jr, Dunaway RG, Benson ML (1997) The social consequences of self-control: testing the general theory of crime. Criminology 35:475–500

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson MR, Hirschi T (1990) A general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Google Scholar 

  • Grasmick HG, Tittle CR, Bursik RJ, Arneklev BJ (1993) Testing the core empirical implications of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime. J Res Crime Delinq 30:5–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschi T (2004) Self-control and crime. In: Baumeister RF, Vohs KD (eds) The handbook of self-regulation. Guilford, New York, NY

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirschi T, Gottfredson MR (1993) Commentary: testing the general theory of crime. J Res Crime Delinq 30:47–54

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hochstetler A, DeLisi M, Puhrmann AM (2007) Toward an integrated model of offending frequency: a replication study. Justice Q 24:582–599

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jussim L, Osgood DW (1989) Influence and similarity among friends: an integrative model applied to incarcerated adolescents. Soc Psychol Q 52:98–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kandel D (1996) The parental and peer contexts of adolescent deviance: an algebra of interpersonal influences. J Drug Issues 26:289–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenny DA, Kashy DA, Cook WL (2006) Dyadic data analysis. Guilford, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkamp CT, Cullen FT, Pratt TC (2003) Replicating Sampson and Groves’s test of social-disorganization theory: revisiting a criminological classic. J Res Crime Delinq 40:351–373

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsueda RL, Anderson K (1998) The dynamics of delinquent peers and delinquent behavior. Criminology 36:269–308

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meldrum RC, Boman JH (2013) Similarities and differences between perceptions of peer delinquency, peer self-reported delinquency, and respondent delinquency: an analysis of friendship dyads. J Crim Justice 41:395–406

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meldrum RC, Young JTN, Weerman FM (2009) Reconsidering the effect of self-control and delinquent peers: implications of measurement for theoretical significance. J Res Crime Delinq 46:353–376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt TC, Cullen FT (2000) The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: a meta-analysis. Criminology 38:932–964

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pratt TC, Cullen FT, Sellers CS, Winfree LT, Madensen TD, Daigle LE, Fearn NE, Gau JM (2010) The empirical status of social learning theory: a meta-analysis. Justice Q 27:765–802

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinstein MJ, Wang SS (2005) False consensus and adolescent peer contagion: examining discrepancies between perceptions and actual reported levels of friends’ deviant and health risk behaviors. J Abnorm Child Psychol 33:293–306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rebellon CJ, Modecki KL (2014) Accounting for projection bias in models of delinquent peer influence: the utility and limits of latent variable approaches. J Quant Criminol 30:163–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saris WE, Aalberts C (2003) Different explanations for correlated disturbance terms in MTMM studies. Struct Equ Model 10:193–213

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Urberg KA, Shyu S, Liang J (1990) Peer influence in adolescent cigarette smoking. Addict Behav 15:247–255

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward JT, Gibson CL, Boman J, Leite WL (2010) Assessing the validity of the retrospective behavioral self-control scale: is the general theory of crime stronger than the evidence suggests? Crim Justice Behav 37:336–357

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warr M (2002) Companions in crime: the social aspects of criminal conduct. Cambridge University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Weerman FM, Smeenk WH (2005) Peer similarity in delinquency for different types of friends: a comparison using two measurement methods. Criminology 43:499–523

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young JTN, Weerman FM (2013) Delinquency as a consequence of misperception: overestimation of friends’ delinquent behavior and mechanisms of social influence. Soc Probl 60:334–356

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young JTN, Rebellon CJ, Barnes JC, Weerman FM (2014) Unpacking the black box of ‘how peers matter’ using the integrative model of personal influence. Criminology 52:60–86

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John H. Boman IV.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Boman, J.H., Rebellon, C.J. & Meldrum, R.C. Can Item-Level Error Correlations Correct for Projection Bias in Perceived Peer Deviance Measures? A Research Note. J Quant Criminol 32, 89–102 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-015-9255-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-015-9255-8

Keywords

Navigation