Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Genetic and Environmental Overlap between Low Self-Control and Delinquency

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Quantitative Criminology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Low self-control has emerged as a consistent and strong predictor of antisocial and delinquent behaviors. Using the twin subsample of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health), genetic analyses were conducted to examine the genetic and environmental contributions to low self-control and offending as well as to their relationship with one another. The results revealed that low self-control and criminal behaviors are influenced by genetic and nonshared environmental factors with the effects of shared environmental factors being negligible. In addition, the co-variation between low self-control and criminal behaviors appears to be largely due to common genetic and nonshared environmental factors operating on both phenotypes. The implications of these findings on the current understanding of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. There was an approximately even distribution of males (48%) and females (47%), with 6% of the cases missing information on gender. Also, approximately 60% of the sample was White, 23% was Black, 10% was classified as other, and 6% was unknown.

  2. The model is deemed a good fit to the data if the Comparative Fix Index (CFI) is ≥0.95, the Tucker Lewis Index (TLI) is ≥0.95, and the Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation (RMSEA) is ≤0.06.

  3. The mean adjusted weighted least squares estimator is appropriate for categorical indicators, such as those included in the measures of self-control in this study.

  4. A covariance was added between the error terms for the measures of “getting into trouble with teachers” and “getting into trouble with other students” at wave I.

  5. A covariance was added between the error terms for the measures of “getting into trouble with teachers” and “getting into trouble with other students” at wave II.

  6. This is accomplished by estimating a series of models where the factor loadings and thresholds are fixed or freed, and then the models are compared with the Santorra Bentler difference of Chi-square test.

  7. More specifically, the presence of weak factorial invariance suggests that the interpretation of the items does not vary over time, and that the factors are in the same metric over time (i.e., a one unit change in the factor score corresponds the same amount of change in the observed variable over time). The presence of invariance in the thresholds suggests that the response choices are interpreted the same across time.

  8. A covariance was added between the error terms for the measures of “look for exciting things to do” and “try new things just for fun or thrills” at wave III.

  9. Many studies have found self-control to be moderately to highly stable (Arneklev et al. 1998; Hay and Forrest 2006). The low between wave correlations reported here could be the result of several different factors, including but not limited to, (1) unique sampling variance specific to the Add Health, (2) the time elapsed between waves—the time between wave I and wave III is 7 years, (3) items included in the measures at waves I and II were not the same as those included at wave III, and/or (4) compounded measurement error across waves, as error over time reduces stability estimates.

  10. With a few exceptions.

  11. We also conducted our analyses using an expanded measure of self-control developed by Beaver et al. (2009a, b). The results from those additional analyses virtually mirrored those presented here and are available upon request. Specifically, across the three waves, genetic factors accounted for 40–45, 38–56, 35–51, 30–56% of the variance in self-control, overall delinquency, nonviolent delinquency, and violent delinquency, respectively. Furthermore, the estimates showed that, across the three waves, common genetic factors accounted for 45–80, 46–79, and 26–75% of the covariance between low self-control and overall delinquency, between low self-control and nonviolent delinquency, and between low self-control and violent delinquency, respectively.

  12. Our results differ slightly from the meta-analyses by Rhee and Waldman (2002) and Burt (2009) who reported that shared environmental influences account for 16 and 18% of the variance in antisocial behaviors and nonaggressive forms of antisocial behaviors, respectively.

  13. MAOA is an enzyme that breaks down neurotransmitters, such as dopamine, serotonin, and epinephrine.

  14. However, it is possible that the effects of shared environment were underestimated due to the lack of inclusion of other siblings of varying degrees of genetic relatedness. Using an extended-twin design could potentially increase the statistical power to detect shared environmental influences (Posthuma and Boomsma 2000). Also, our results differ slightly from the meta-analysis by Rhee and Waldman (2002) which revealed that 41, 16, and 43% of the variance in antisocial behaviors is attributed to genetic, shared, and nonshared environmental factors, respectively.

References

  • Akaike H (1987) Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika 52(3):317–332

    Google Scholar 

  • Arcos-Burgos M, Castellanos FX, Konecki D, Lopera F, Pineda D, Palacio JD, Rapoport JL, Berg K, Bailey-Wilson J, Muenke M (2004) Pedigree disequilibrium test (PDT) replicates association and linkage between DRD4 and ADHD in multigenerational and extended pedigrees from a genetic isolate. Mol Psychiatry 9:252–259

    Google Scholar 

  • Arneklev BJ, Grasmick HG, Tittle CR, Bursik RJ (1993) Low self-control and imprudent behavior. J Quant Criminol 9(3):225–247

    Google Scholar 

  • Arneklev BJ, Cochran JK, Gainey RR (1998) Testing gottfredson and Hirschi’s “low self-control” stability hypothesis: an exploratory study. Am J Crim Justice 23(1):107–127

    Google Scholar 

  • Arseneault L, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor A, Rijsdijk FV, Jaffee SR et al (2003) Strong genetic effects on cross-situational antisocial behaviour among 5-year-old children according to mothers, teachers, examiner-observers, and twins’ self-reports. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 44(6):832–848

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley RA (1997) ADHD and the nature of self-control. Guilford, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley RA (2006) Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, 3rd edn.: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment. Guilford, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkley RA, Cook EH, Diamond A, Zametkin A, Thapar A, Teeter A et al (2002) International consensus statement on ADHD. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 5:89–111

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron SW (2003) Self-control, social consequences, and criminal behavior: street youth and the general theory of crime. J Res Crime Delinq 40(4):403–425

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartels M, Hudziak JJ, Van den Oord EJCG, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Rietveld MJH, Boomsma DI (2003) Co-occurrence of aggressive behavior and rule-breaking behavior at age 12: multi-rater analyses. Behav Genet 33:607–621

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM (2008) Nonshared environmental influences on adolescent delinquency involvement and adult criminal behavior. Criminology 46(2):341–369

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, Delisi M, Vaughn MG, Wright JP, Boutwell BB (2008a) The relationship between self-control and language: evidence of a shared etiological pathway. Criminology 46(4):939–970

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, Wright JP (2005) Evaluating the effects of birth complications on low self-control in a sample of twins. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol 49:450–471

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, Wright JP, DeLisi M (2008b) Delinquent peer group formation: evidence of a gene X environment correlation. J Genet Psychol 169(3):227–244

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, Wright JP, DeLisi M, Vaughn MG (2008c) Desistance from delinquency: the marriage effect revisited and extended. Soc Sci Res 37(3):736–752

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, Wright JP, DeLisi M, Vaughn MG (2008d) Genetic influences on the stability of low self-control: results from a longitudinal sample of twins. J Crim Justice 36(6):478–485

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, Eagle Schutt J, Boutwell BB, Ratchford M, Roberts K, Barnes JC (2009a) Genetic and environmental influences on levels of self-control and delinquent peer affiliation: results from a longitudinal sample of adolescent twins. Crim Justice Behav 36(1):41–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, Ratchford M, Ferguson CJ (2009b) Evidence of genetic and environmental effects on the development of self-control. Crim Justice Behav 36:1158–1172

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver KM, DeLisi M, Mears DP, Stewart E (2009c) Low self-control and contact with the criminal justice system in a nationally representative sample of males. Justice Q 26(4):695–715

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker K, Laucht M, El-Faddagh M, Schmidt MH (2005) The dopamine D4 receptor gene exon III polymorphism is associated with novelty seeking in 15-year-old males from a high-risk community sample. J Neural Transm 112:847–858

    Google Scholar 

  • Beitchman JH, Mik HM, Ehtesham S, Douglas L, Kennedy JL (2004) MAOA and persistent, pervasive childhood aggression. Mol Psychiatry 9:546–547

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohman M, Cloninger CR, Sigvardsson S, von Knorring AL (1982) Predisposition to petty criminality in Swedish adoptees. I. Genetic and environmental heterogeneity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 39(11):1233–1241

    Google Scholar 

  • Boisvert D, Wright JP (2008) Nonshared environmental influences on sibling differences in externalizing problem behavior. Crim Justice Behav 35(7):863–878

    Google Scholar 

  • Boutwell BB, Beaver KM (2008) A biosocial explanation of delinquency abstention. Crim Behav Mental Health 18(1):59–74

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody G, Stoneman Z (1994) Sibling relationships and their association with parental differential treatment. J Family Psychol 8:129–142

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody G, Stoneman Z, Burke M (1987) Child temperaments, maternal differential behavior and sibling relationships. Dev Psychol 23:354–362

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA (2009) Are there meaningful etiological differences within antisocial behavior? Results of a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 29(2):163–178

    Google Scholar 

  • Burt SA, Krueger RF, McGue M, Iacono WG (2001) Sources of covariation among attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and conduct disorder: the importance of shared environment. J Abnorm Psychol 110(4):516–525

    Google Scholar 

  • Burton VS Jr, Cullen FT, Evans TD, Alarid LF, Dunaway RG (1998) Gender, self-control, and crime. J Res Crime Delinq 35(2):123–147

    Google Scholar 

  • Button TM, Scourfield J, Martin N, McGuffin P (2004) Do aggressive and non-aggressive antisocial behaviors in adolescents result from the same genetic and environmental effects? Am J Med Genet 129B:59–63

    Google Scholar 

  • Cauffman E, Steinberg L, Piquero AR (2005) Psychological, neurophsychological and physiological correlates of serious antisocial behavior in adolescence: the role of self control. Criminology 43(1):133–176

    Google Scholar 

  • Chantala K, Tabor J (1999) Strategies to perform a design-based analysis using the Add Health data. Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Available at: http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/files/weight1.pdf

  • Cloninger CR, Gottesman II (1987) Genetic and environmental factors in antisocial behavior disorders. In: Mednick SA, Moffitt TE, Stack SA (eds) The causes of crime: new biological approaches. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 92–109

    Google Scholar 

  • Cochran JK, Wood PB, Sellers CS, Wilkerson W, Chamlin MB (1998) Academic dishonesty and low self-control: an empirical test of a general theory of crime. Deviant Behav 19:227–255

    Google Scholar 

  • Comings DE, Gade-Andavolu R, Gonzalez N, Wu S, Muhleman D, Blake H, Chiu F, Wang E, Farwell K, Darakjy S, Baker R, Dietz G, Saucier G, MacMurray JP (2000a) Multivariate analysis of associations of 42 genes in ADHD, ODD, and conduct disorder. Clin Genet 58:31–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Comings DE, Gade-Andavolu R, Gonzalez N, Wu S, Muhleman D, Blake H, Dietz G, Saucier G, MacMurray JP (2000b) Comparison of the role of dopamine, serotonin, and noradrenaline genes in ADHD, ODD and conduct disorder: multivariate regression analysis of 20 genes. Clin Genet 57:178–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Conger KJ, Conger RD (1994) Differential parenting and change in sibling differences in delinquency. J Fam Psychol 8:287–302

    Google Scholar 

  • Cullen FT, Unnever JD, Wright JP, Beaver KM (2008) Parenting and self-control. In: Goode E (ed) Out of control: assessing the general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Stanford, pp 61–74

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniels D, Dunn J, Furstenberg FF Jr, Plomin R (1985) Environmental differences within the family and adjustment differences within pairs of adolescent siblings. Child Dev 56:764–774

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLisi M (2001) It’s all in the record: assessing self-control theory with an offender sample. Crim Justice Rev 26(1):1–16

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLisi M, Vaughn MG (2011) The importance of neuropsychological deficits relating to self-control and temperament to the prevention of serious antisocial behavior. Int J Child Youth Fam Stud 1&2:12–35

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLisi M, Beaver KM, Wright JP, Vaughn MG (2008) The etiology of criminal onset: the enduring salience of nature and nurture. J Crim Justice 36(3):217–223

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond A, Barnett WS, Thomas J, Munro S (2007) Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science 318(5855):1387–1388

    Google Scholar 

  • Dionne G, Tremblay R, Boivin M, Laplante D, Perusse D (2003) Physical aggression and expressive vocabulary in 19-month-old twins. Dev Psychol 39(2):261–273

    Google Scholar 

  • Duckworth AL, Seligman ME (2005) Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychol Sci 16(12):939–944

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunn J, Stocker C, Plomin R (1990) Nonshared experiences within the family: correlates of behavioral problems in middle childhood. Dev Psychopathol 2:113–126

    Google Scholar 

  • Eaves LJ, Silberg JL, Meyer JM, Maes HH, Simonoff E, Pickles A et al (1997) Genetics and developmental psychopathology: 2. The main effects of genes and environment on behavioral problems in the Virginia twin study of adolescent behavioral development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38(8):965–980

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelbrock C, Rende R, Plomin R, Thompson LA (1995) A twin study of competence and problem behavior in childhood and early adolescence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 36(5):775–785

    Google Scholar 

  • El-Faddagh M, Laucht M, Maras A, Vöhringer L, Schmidt MH (2004) Association of dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in a high-risk community sample: a longitudinal study from birth to 11 years of age. J Neural Transm 111:883–889

    Google Scholar 

  • Forde D, Kennedy L (1997) Risky lifestyles, routine activities, and the general theory of crime. Justice Q 14(2):265–294

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank Y, Pergolizzi RG, Perilla MJ (2004) Dopamine D4 receptor gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr Neurol 31:345–348

    Google Scholar 

  • Fulker DW, Eysenck SBG, Zuckerman M (1980) A genetic and environmental analysis of sensation seeking. J Res Pers 14(2):261–281

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelhorn HL, Stallings MC, Young SE, Corley RP, Rhee SH, Hewitt JK (2005) Genetic and environmental influences on conduct disorder: symptom, domain and full-scale analyses. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46:580–591

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs J, Giever D (1995) Self-control and its manifestations among university students: an empirical test of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory. Justice Q 12(2):231–255

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs JJ, Giever D, Higgins GE (2003) A test of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory using structural equation modeling. Crim Justice Behav 30(4):441–458

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilger JW, Pennington BF, DeFries JC (1992) A twin study of the etiology of comorbidity: attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and dyslexia. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 31(2):343–348

    Google Scholar 

  • Gjone H, Stevenson J, Sundet JM (1996) Genetic influence on parent-reported attention-related problems in a Norwegian general population twin sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 35(5):588–598

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein RB, Prescott CA, Kendler KS (2001) Genetic and environmental factors in conduct problems and adult antisocial behavior among adult female twins. J Nerv Ment Dis 189(4):201–209

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson M, Hirschi T (1990) A general theory of crime. Stanford University Press, Palo Alto

    Google Scholar 

  • Grasmick HG, Tittle CR, Bursik RJ, Arneklev B (1993) Testing the core empirical implications of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime. J Res Crime Delinq 30:5–29

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg MT, Kusche C, Mihalic SF (2006) Promoting alternative thinking strategies (PATHS): blueprints for violence prevention. Center for the Study and Prevention of Violence, Boulder

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris KM, Halpern CT, Smolen A, Haberstick BC (2006) The national longitudinal study of adolescent health (Add Health) twin data. Twin Res Human Genet 9(6):988–997

    Google Scholar 

  • Hay C, Forrest W (2006) The development of self-control: examining self-control theory’s stability thesis. Criminology 44(4):739–774

    Google Scholar 

  • Haynie DL (2001) Delinquent peers revisited: does network structure matter. Am J Sociol 106:1013–1057

    Google Scholar 

  • Haynie DL (2002) Friendship networks and delinquency: the relative nature of peer delinquency. J Quant Criminol 18:99–134

    Google Scholar 

  • Haynie DL, Osgood DW (2005) Reconsidering peers and delinquency: how do peers matter. Soc Forces 84:1109–1130

    Google Scholar 

  • Haynie DL, Giordano PC, Manning WD, Longmore MA (2005) Adolescent romantic relationships and delinquent involvement. Criminology 43(1):177–210

    Google Scholar 

  • Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Iacono WG, McGue M, Patrick CJ (2004) Family transmission and heritability of externalizing disorders: a twin-family study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 61(9):922–928

    Google Scholar 

  • Hudziak JJ, Rudiger LP, Neale MC, Heath AC, Todd RD (2000) A twin study of inattentive, aggressive, and anxious/depressed behaviors. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 39(4):469–476

    Google Scholar 

  • Hur YM, Bouchard TJ (1997) The genetic correlation between impulsivity and sensation seeking traits. Behav Genet 27(5):455–463

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones S, Quisenberry N (2004) The general theory of crime: how general is it? Deviant Behav 25(5):401–426

    Google Scholar 

  • Junger M, Tremblay RE (1999) Self-control, accidents, and crime. Crim Justice Behav 26(4):485–501

    Google Scholar 

  • Junger M, West R, Timman R (2001) Crime and risky behavior in traffic: an example of cross-situational consistency. J Res Crime Delinq 38(4):439–459

    Google Scholar 

  • Keane C, Maxim PS, Teevan JJ (1993) Drinking and driving, self-control, and gender: testing a general theory of crime. J Res Crime Delinq 30(1):30–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirley A, Lowe N, Mullins C, McCarron M, Daly G, Waldman I, Fitzgerald M, Gill M, Hawi Z (2004) Phenotype studies of the DRD4 polymorphisms in ADHD: association with oppositional defiant disorder and positive family history. Am J Med Genet 131B:38–42

    Google Scholar 

  • Knopik VS, Sparrow EP, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK, Hudziak JJ, Reich W, Slutske WS, Grant JD, McLaughlin TL, Todorov A, Todd RD, Heath AC (2005) Contributions of parental alcoholism, prenatal substance exposure, and genetic transmission to child ADHD risk: a female twin study. Psychol Med 35(5):625–635

    Google Scholar 

  • Koller G, Bondy B, Preuss UW, Bottlender M, Soyka M (2003) No association between a polymorphism in the promoter region of the MAOA gene with antisocial personality traits in alcoholics. Alcohol Alcohol 38:31–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Koopmans JR, Boomsma DI, Heath AC, Doornen LJP (1995) A multivariate genetic analysis of sensation seeking. Behav Genet 25(4):349–356

    Google Scholar 

  • Kotler M, Manor I, Sever Y, Eisenberg J, Cohen H, Ebstein R, Tyano S (2000) Failure to replicate an excess of the long dopamine D4 exon III repeat polymorphism in ADHD in a family-based study. Am J Med Genet 96:278–281

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger RF, Hicks BM, McGue M (2001) Altruism and antisocial behavior: independent tendencies, unique personality correlates, distinct etiologies. Psychol Sci 12(5):397–402

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuntsi J, Gayan J, Stevenson J (2000) Parents’ and teachers’ ratings of problem behaviours in children: genetic and contrast effects. Twin Res Human Genet 3(4):251–258

    Google Scholar 

  • Laucht M, Becker K, El-Faddagh M, Hohm E, Schmidt MH (2005) Association of the DRD4 exon III polymorphism with smoking in fifteen-year-olds: a mediating role for novelty seeking? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44:477–484

    Google Scholar 

  • Lawson DC, Turic D, Langley K, Pay HM, Govan CF, Norton N, Hamshere ML, Owen MJ, O’Donovan MC, Thapar A (2003) Association analysis of monoamine oxidase A and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet 116B:84–89

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee HJ, Lee HS, Kim YK, Kim L, Lee MS, Jung IK, Suh KY, Kim S (2003) D2 and D4 dopamine receptor gene polymorphisms and personality traits in a young Korean population. Am J Med Genet 121B:44–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Longshore D, Turner S (1998) Self-control and criminal opportunity: cross-sectional test of the general theory of crime. Crim Justice Behav 25(1):81–98

    Google Scholar 

  • Lu RB, Lin WW, Lee JF, Ko HC, Shih JC (2003) Neither antisocial personality disorder nor antisocial alcoholism is associated with the MAO-A gene in Hans Chinese Males. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 27:889–893

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynam D, Moffitt T, Stouthamer-Loeber M (1993) Explaining the relationship between IQ and delinquency: class, race, test motivation, school failure, or self-control. J Abnorm Psychol 102(2):187–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Manor I, Tyano S, Mel E, Eisenberg J, Bachner-Melman R, Kotler M, Ebstein RP (2002) Family-based and association studies of monoamine oxidase A and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): preferential transmission of the long promoter-region repeat and its association with impaired performance on a continuous performance test (TOVA). Mol Psychiatry 7:626–632

    Google Scholar 

  • Manuk SB, Flory JD, Ferrell RE, Mann JJ, Muldoon MF (2000) A regulatory polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase-A gene may be associated with variability in aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity. Psychiatry Res 95:9–23

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin MG, Eaves LJ, Kearsey MJ, Davies P (1978) The power of the classical twin study. Heredity 40:97–116

    Google Scholar 

  • McGloin JM, O’Neill Shermer L (2009) Self-control and deviant peer network structure. J Res Crime Delinq 46(1):35–72

    Google Scholar 

  • McGloin JM, Pratt TC, Maahs J (2004) Rethinking the IQ–delinquency relationship: a longitudinal analysis of multiple theoretical models. Justice Q 21(3):603–635

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuire S, Dunn J, Plomin R (1995) Maternal differential treatment of siblings and children’s behavioral problems: a longitudinal study. Dev Psychopathol 7:515–528

    Google Scholar 

  • McHale SM, Pawletko TM (1992) Differential treatment of siblings in two family contexts. Child Dev 63:68–81

    Google Scholar 

  • Mednick SA, Gabrielli WF Jr, Hutchings B (1984) Genetic influences in criminal convictions: evidence from an adoption cohort. Science 224(4651):891–894

    Google Scholar 

  • Miles DR, van den Bree MB, Pickens RW (2002) Sex differences in shared genetic and environmental influences between conduct disorder symptoms and marijuana use in adolescents. Am J Med Genet 114(2):159–168

    Google Scholar 

  • Mill J, Xu X, Ronald A, Curran S, Price T, Knight J, Craig I, Sham P, Plomin R, Asherson P (2005) Quantitative trait locus analysis of candidate gene alleles associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with five genes: DRD4, DAT1, DRD5, SNAP-25, and 5HT1B. Am J Med Genet 133B:68–73

    Google Scholar 

  • Moffitt TE (2005) Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behaviors: evidence from behavioral-genetic research. Adv Genet 55:41–104

    Google Scholar 

  • Moilanen I, Ebeling H (1998) To be born as a twin–risks and sequelae. Int J Circumpolar Health 57:138–147

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadder TS, Silberg JL, Eaves LJ, Maes HH, Meyer JM (1998) Genetic effects on ADHD symptomatology in 7-to 13-year-old twins: results from a telephone survey. Behav Genet 28(2):83–99

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadder TS, Rutter M, Silberg JL, Maes HH, Eaves LJ (2002) Genetic effects on the variation and covariation of attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional-defiant disorder/conduct disorder (ODD/CD) symptomatologies across informant and occasion of measurement. Psychol Med 32(1):39–53

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakhaie MR, Silverman RA, LaGrange TC (2000) Self-control and social control: an examination of gender, ethnicity, class and delinquency. Canadian J Sociol 25(1):35–59

    Google Scholar 

  • Neale MC, Cardon LR (1992) Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Kluwer, Dordrecht

    Google Scholar 

  • Nikolas M, Burt SA (2010) Genetic and environmental influences on ADHD symptom dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity: a meta-analysis. J Abnorm Psychol 119:1–17

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearce LD, Haynie DL (2003) Intergenerational religious dynamics and adolescent delinquency. Soc Forces 82:1553–1572

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrone D, Sullivan CJ, Pratt TC, Margaryan S (2004) Parental efficacy, self-control, and delinquency: a test of a general theory of crime on a nationally representative sample of youth. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol 48(3):298–312

    Google Scholar 

  • Piquero A, Tibbetts S (1996) Specifying the direct and indirect effects of low self-control and situational factors in offenders’ decision making: toward a more complete model of rational offending. Justice Q 13(3):481–510

    Google Scholar 

  • Plomin R (1990) Nature and nurture: an introduction to human behavioral genetics. Brooks/Cole Publishing Company, Pacific Grove

    Google Scholar 

  • Ponce G, Jimenez-Arriero MA, Rubio G, Hoenicka J, Ampuero I, Ramos JA, Palomo T (2003) The A1 allele of the DRD2 gene (TaqI A polymorphisms) is associated with antisocial personality in a sample of alcohol-dependent patients. Eur Psychiatry 18:356–360

    Google Scholar 

  • Posthuma D, Boomsma DI (2000) A note on the statistical power in extended twin designs. Behav Genet 30:147–158

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratt TC, Cullen FT (2000) The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: a meta-analysis. Criminology 38:931–964

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratt TC, Blevins K, Daigle LE, Cullen FT, Unnever JD (2002) The relationship of ADHD to crime and delinquency: a meta-analysis. Int J Police Sci Manag 4(4):344–360

    Google Scholar 

  • Price TS, Simonoff E, Waldman I, Asherson P, Plomin R (2001) Hyperactivity in preschool children is highly heritable. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 12:1362–1364

    Google Scholar 

  • Qiujin Q, Wang Y, Zhou R, Yang L, Faraone SV (2004) Family-based and case-control association studies of DRD4 and DAT1 polymorphisms in Chinese attention deficit hyperactivity disorder patients suggest long repeats contribute to genetic risk for the disorder. Am J Med Genet 123B:84–89

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhee SH, Waldman ID (2002) Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: a meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychol Bull 128:490–529

    Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld MJH, Hudziak JJ, Bartels M, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Boomsma DI (2003) Heritability of attention problems in children: cross-sectional results from a study of twins, age 3 to 12 years. Neuropsychiatr Genet 1176:102–113

    Google Scholar 

  • Rietveld MJH, Hudziak JJ, Bartels M, Beijsterveldt CEM, Boomsma DI (2004) Heritability of attention problems in children: longitudinal results from a study of twins age 3 to 12. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 45:577–588

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross RR, Hilborn J (2008) Rehabilitating rehabilitation: neurocriminology for treatment of antisocial behaviors. Air Training and Publications, Ottawa

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter M, Simonoff E, Silberg J (1993) How informative are twin studies of child psychopathology? In: Bouchard T, Propping P (eds) Twins as a tool of behavior genetics. Wiley, Berlin, pp 179–194

    Google Scholar 

  • Saudino KJ, Cherny SS, Plomin R (2000) Parent ratings of temperament in twins: explaining the ‘too low’ DZ correlations. Twin Res Human Genet 3(4):224–233

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt LG, Sander T, Kuhn S, Smolka M, Rommelspacher H, Samochowiec J, Lesch KP (2000) Different allele distribution of a regulatory MAOA gene promoter polymorphism in antisocial and anxious-depressive alcoholics. J Neural Transm 107:681–689

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz S, Mrazek DA (2001) Genetic and environmental influences on the associations between attention problems and other problem behaviors. Twin Res Human Genet 4(6):453–458

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz S, Fulker DW, Mrazek DA (1995) Problem behavior in early and middle childhood: an initial behavior genetic analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 36(8):1443–1458

    Google Scholar 

  • Schweitzer JB, Sulzer-Azaroff B (1995) Self-control in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: effects of added stimulation and time. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 36:671–686

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman DK, McGue MK, Iacono WG (1997) Twin concordance for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a comparison of teachers’ and mothers’ reports. Am J Psychiatry 154(4):532–535

    Google Scholar 

  • Silberg J, Rutter M, Meyer J, Maes H, Hewitt J, Simonoff E et al (1996) Genetic and environmental influences on the covariation between hyperactivity and conduct disturbance in juvenile twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discipl 37(7):803–816

    Google Scholar 

  • Simsek M, Al-Sharbati M, Al-Adawi S, Ganguly SS, Lawatia K (2005) Association of the risk allele of dopamine transporter gene (DAT1*10) in Omani male children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin Biochem 38:739–742

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith KM, Daly M, Fischer M, Yiannoutsos CT, Bauer L, Barkley R, Navia BA (2003) Association of the dopamine beta hydroxylase gene with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: genetic analysis of the Milwaukee longitudinal study. Am J Med Genet 119B:77–85

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer TJ, Biederman J, Wilens TE, Faraone SV (2002) Overview and neurobiology of attention-deficit/hyper-activity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 63(Suppl 12):3–9

    Google Scholar 

  • Stocker CM (1993) Siblings’ adjustment in middle childhood: links with mother-child relationships. J Appl Dev Psychol 14:485–499

    Google Scholar 

  • Stocker CM (1995) Differences in mothers’ and fathers’ relationship with siblings: links with children’s behavior problems. Dev Psychopathol 7:499–513

    Google Scholar 

  • Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL (2004) High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J Pers 72(2):271–324

    Google Scholar 

  • Thapar A, Hervas A, McGuffin P (1995) Childhood hyperactivity scores are highly heritable and show sibling competition effects: twin study evidence. Behav Genet 25(6):537–544

    Google Scholar 

  • Tibbetts SG, Piquero A (1999) The influence of gender, low birth weight, and disadvantaged environment in predicting early onset of offending: a test of Moffitt’s interactional hypothesis. Criminology 37:843–878

    Google Scholar 

  • Tsai SJ, Hong CJ, Yu YW, Chen TJ (2004) Association study of catechol-O-methyltransferase gene and dopamine D4 receptor gene polymorphisms and personality traits in healthy young Chinese females. Neuropsychobiology 50:153–156

    Google Scholar 

  • Unnever JD, Cornell DG (2003) Bullying, self-control, and ADHD. J Interpers Viol 18(2):129–147

    Google Scholar 

  • Unnever JD, Cullen FT, Pratt TC (2003) Parental management, ADHD, and delinquent involvement: reassessing Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory. Justice Q 20:471–500

    Google Scholar 

  • van den Oord E, Verhulst FC, Boomsma DI (1996) A genetic study of maternal and paternal ratings of problem behaviors in 3-year-old twins. J Abnorm Psychol 105(3):349–357

    Google Scholar 

  • van der Ende J, Verhulst FC (2005) Informant, gender and age differences in ratings of adolescent problem behaviour. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 14:117–126

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaske J, Galyean K, Cullen FT (2011) Toward a biosocial theory of offender rehabilitation: why does cognitive-behavioral therapy work? J Crim Justice 39:90–102

    Google Scholar 

  • Vazsonyi AT, Crosswhite JM (2004) A test of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime in African American adolescents. J Res Crime Delinq 41(4):407–432

    Google Scholar 

  • Vazsonyi AT, Pickering LE, Junger M, Hessing D (2001) An empirical test of a general theory of crime: a four-nation comparative study of self-control and the prediction of deviance. J Res Crime Delinq 38(2):91–131

    Google Scholar 

  • Vierikko E, Pulkkinen L, Kaprio J, Rose RJ (2004) Genetic and environmental influences on the relationship between aggression and hyperactivity-impulsivity as rated by teachers and parents. Twin Res Hum Genet 7(3):261–274

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh A (2002) Biosocial criminology: introduction and integration. Anderson Publishing, Cincinnati

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright JP, Beaver KM (2005) Do parents matter in creating self-control in their children? A genetically informed test of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s theory of low self-control. Criminology 43:1169–1202

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright J, Beaver K, Delisi M, Vaughn M (2008) Evidence of negligible parenting influences on self-control, delinquent peers, and delinquency in a sample of twins. Justice Q 25(3):544–569

    Google Scholar 

  • Young SE, Stallings MC, Corley RP, Krauter KS, Hewitt JK (2000) Genetic and environmental influences on behavioral disinhibition. Am J Med Genet 96:684–695

    Google Scholar 

  • Young SE, Smolen A, Corley RP, Krauter KS, DeFries JC, Crowley TJ, Hewitt JK (2002) Dopamine transporter polymorphism associated with externalizing behavior problems in children. Am J Med Genet 114:144–149

    Google Scholar 

  • Young S, Chick K, Gudjonsson G (2010) A preliminary evaluation of reasoning and rehabilitation 2 in mentally disordered offenders (R&R2) across two secure forensic settings in the United Kingdom. J Forensic Psychiatry Psychol 21:336–349

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler C, Schmitz S, Fulker D, Robinson J, Emde R (1996) Behavior problems in 5-year-old monozygotic and dizygotic twins: genetic and environmental influences, patterns of regulation, and internalization of control. Dev Psychopathol 8:103–122

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Danielle Boisvert.

Appendix

Appendix

Low Self-Control at Wave I

  1. 1.

    How often do you have trouble getting along with your teachersa

  2. 2.

    How often do you have trouble paying attention in schoola

  3. 3.

    How often do you have trouble getting your homework done a

  4. 4.

    How often do you have trouble getting along with other studentsa

  5. 5.

    How often did you have trouble keeping your mind on what you were doing, during the past seven daysb

Response Categories: a0 = never, 1 = just a few times, 2 = about once a week, 3 = almost everyday, 4 = everyday. b0 = never or rarely, 1 = sometimes, 2 = a lot of the time, 3 = most of the time or all of the time. (α = 0.71)

Low Self-Control at Wave II

  1. 1.

    How often do you have trouble getting along with your teachersa

  2. 2.

    How often do you have trouble paying attention in schoola

  3. 3.

    How often do you have trouble getting your homework donea

  4. 4.

    How often do you have trouble getting along with other studentsa

  5. 5.

    How often did you have trouble keeping your mind on what you were doing, during the past seven daysb

Response Categories: a0 = never, 1 = just a few times, 2 = about once a week, 3 = almost everyday, 4 = everyday. b0 = never or rarely, 1 = sometimes, 2 = a lot of the time, 3 = most of the time or all of the time. (α = 0.67)

Low Self-Control at Wave III

  1. 1.

    I often try new things just for fun or thrills, even if most people think they are a waste of time

  2. 2.

    When nothing new is happening, I usually start looking for something exciting

  3. 3.

    I often do things based on how I feel at the moment

  4. 4.

    I sometimes get so excited that I lose control of myself

  5. 5.

    I like it when people can do whatever they want, without strict rules and regulations

  6. 6.

    I often follow my instincts, without thinking through all the details

  7. 7.

    I change my interest a lot, because my attention often shifts to something else

Response Categories: 0 = not true, 1 = a little true, 2 = somewhat true, 3 = pretty true, 4 = very true. (α = 0.84)

Overall Delinquency at Wave I and II

In the past 12 months, how often did you:

  1. 1.

    Get into a serious physical fighta

  2. 2.

    Hurt someone badly enough to need bandages or care from a doctor or nursea

  3. 3.

    Use or threaten to use a weapon to get something from someonea

  4. 4.

    Take part in a fight where a group of your friends were against another groupa

  5. 5.

    Pull a knife or a gun on someoneb

  6. 6.

    You shot or stabbed someoneb

  7. 7.

    Paint graffiti or signs on someone else’s property or in a public placea

  8. 8.

    Deliberately damage property that didn’t belong to youa

  9. 9.

    Take something from a store without paying for ita

  10. 10.

    Drive a car without its owner’s permissiona

  11. 11.

    Steal something worth more than $50a

  12. 12.

    Go into a house or building to steal somethinga

  13. 13.

    Sell marijuana or other drugsa

  14. 14.

    Steal something worth less than $50a

Response Categories: a0 = never, 1 = one or two times, 2 = three or four times, 3 = five or more times. b0 = never, 1 = once, 2 = more than once. (wave I α = 0.84; wave II α = 0.81)

Overall Offending at Wave III

  1. 1.

    Use a weapon in a fighta

  2. 2.

    Hurt someone badly enough to need bandages or care from a doctor or nursea

  3. 3.

    Use or threaten to use a weapon to get something from someonea

  4. 4.

    Take part in a fight where a group of your friends were against another groupa

  5. 5.

    Pull a knife or a gun on someoneb

  6. 6.

    You shot or stabbed someoneb

  7. 7.

    Deliberately damage property that didn’t belong to youa

  8. 8.

    Steal something worth more than $50a

  9. 9.

    Go into a house or building to steal somethinga

  10. 10.

    Sell marijuana or other drugsa

  11. 11.

    Steal something worth less than $50a

  12. 12.

    Buy, sell, or hold stolen propertya

  13. 13.

    Use someone else’s credit or bank card without their permission or knowledgea

  14. 14.

    Deliberately write a bad checka

Response Categories: a0 = never, 1 = one or two times, 2 = three or four times, 3 = five or more times. b0 = no, 1 = yes. (α = 0.73)

Violent Delinquency at Waves I and II

In the past 12 months, how often did you:

  1. 1.

    Get into a serious physical fighta

  2. 2.

    Hurt someone badly enough to need bandages or care from a doctor or nursea

  3. 3.

    Use or threaten to use a weapon to get something from someonea

  4. 4.

    Take part in a fight where a group of your friends were against another groupa

  5. 5.

    Pull a knife or a gun on someoneb

  6. 6.

    You shot or stabbed someoneb

Response Categories: a0 = never, 1 = one or two times, 2 = three or four times, 3 = five or more times. b0 = never, 1 = once, 2 = more than once. (wave I α = 0.75; wave II α = 0.72)

Violent Offending at Wave III

In the past 12 months, how often did you:

  1. 1.

    Use a weapon in a fighta

  2. 2.

    Hurt someone badly enough to need bandages or care from a doctor or nursea

  3. 3.

    Use or threaten to use a weapon to get something from someonea

  4. 4.

    Take part in a fight where a group of your friends were against another groupa

  5. 5.

    Pull a knife or a gun on someoneb

  6. 6.

    You shot or stabbed someoneb

Response Categories: a0 = never, 1 = one or two times, 2 = three or four times, 3 = five or more times. b0 = no, 1 = yes. (α = 0.52)

Non-Violent Delinquency at Waves I and II

In the past 12 months, how often did you:

  1. 1.

    Paint graffiti or signs on someone else’s property or in a public place

  2. 2.

    Deliberately damage property that didn’t belong to you

  3. 3.

    Take something from a store without paying for it

  4. 4.

    Drive a car without its owner’s permission

  5. 5.

    Steal something worth more than $50

  6. 6.

    Go into a house or building to steal something

  7. 7.

    Sell marijuana or other drugs

  8. 8.

    Steal something worth less than $50

Response Categories: 0 = never, 1 = one or two times, 2 = three or four times, 3 = five or more times. (wave I α = 0.78; wave II = 0.77)

Non-Violent Offending at Wave III

In the past 12 months, how often did you:

  1. 1.

    Deliberately damage property that didn’t belong to you

  2. 2.

    Steal something worth more than $50

  3. 3.

    Go into a house or building to steal something

  4. 4.

    Sell marijuana or other drugs

  5. 5.

    Steal something worth less than $50

  6. 6.

    Buy, sell, or hold stolen property

  7. 7.

    Use someone else’s credit or bank card without their permission or knowledge

  8. 8.

    Deliberately write a bad check

Response Categories: 0 = never, 1 = one or two times, 2 = three or four times, 3 = five or more times. (α = 0.66)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Boisvert, D., Wright, J.P., Knopik, V. et al. Genetic and Environmental Overlap between Low Self-Control and Delinquency. J Quant Criminol 28, 477–507 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-011-9150-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-011-9150-x

Keywords

Navigation