Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Socioeconomic Status and Parent–Child Relationships Predict Metacognitive Questions to Preschoolers

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The importance of metacognitive language exposure to early educational achievement is widely recognized in the development literature. However, few studies have explored parents’ metacognitive language, while accounting for family SES and stress within the parent–child relationship. This is a preliminary descriptive study to explore metacognitive language to preschoolers during a collaborative task, and explored family SES (parent education and occupation) and stress within the parent–child relationship as predictors. Using partial correlations controlling for child age and language ability, parent education, occupation, and stress associated with parent–child dysfunctional interaction were found to significantly correlate with parents’ metacognitive questions. A hierarchical regression (controlling for child age and language ability) indicated that only parent education levels uniquely predicted questions overall.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abidin, R. R. (1995). Parent stress index (3rd ed.). Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. (1987). Observing interaction: An introduction to sequential analysis (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barry, T., Dunlap, S., Cotten, S., Lochman, J., & Wells, K. (2005). The influence of maternal stress and distress on disruptive behavior problems in boys. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(3), 265–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beeghly, M., Bretherton, I., & Mervis, C. (1986). Mothers’ internal state language to toddlers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(3), 247–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benasich, A., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1996). Maternal attitudes and knowledge of child-rearing: Associations with family and child outcomes. Child Development, 67(3), 1186–1205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bendell, R. D., Stone, W., & Field, T. (1989). Children’s effects on parenting stress in a low income, minority population. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 8, 55–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berk, L., & Spuhl, S. T. (1995). Maternal interaction, private speech and task performance in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 145–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birbili, M., & Karagiorgou, I. (2010). Helping children and their parents ask better questions: An intervention study. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 24(1), 18–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M., Hendricks, C., Haynes, O., & Painter, K. (2007). Maternal sensitivity and child responsiveness: Associations with social context, maternal characteristics, and child characteristics in a multivariate analysis. Infancy, 12(2), 189–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906–921.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke-Stewart, K., & Beck, R. J. (1999). Maternal scaffolding and children’s narrative retelling of a movie story. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 14(3), 409–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damast, A., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1996). Mother-child play: Sequential interactions and the relation between maternal beliefs and behaviors. Child Development, 67(4), 1752–1766.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Rosnay, M., Pons, F., Harris, P. L., & Morrell, J. B. (2004). A lag between understanding false belief and emotion attribution in young children: Relationships with linguistic ability and mothers’ mental-state language. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 197–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J., & Pyers, J. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development, 17(1), 1037–1060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demers, I., Bernier, A., Tarabulsy, G., & Provost, M. A. (2010). Maternal and child characteristics as antecedents of maternal mind-mindedness. Infant Mental Health Journal, 31(1), 94–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D., & Smith, M. (1991). Preschool talk: Patterns of teacher–child interaction in early childhood classrooms. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6(1), 20–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ely, R., Gleason, J., MacGibbon, A., & Zaretsky, E. (2001). Attention to language: Lessons learned at the dinner table. Social Development, 10(3), 355–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, S., Martinez-Pons, M., & Shaham, D. (1995). The relationship of self-efficacy, self-regulation, and collaborative verbal behavior with grades: Preliminary findings. Psychological Reports, 77, 971–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernyhough, C. (2008). Getting Vygotskian about theory of mind: Mediation, dialogue, and the development of social understanding. Developmental Review, 28, 225–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. (2000). Development of children’s knowledge about the mental world. International Journal of Behaviorial Development, 24(1), 15–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forman, D., Aksan, N., & Kochanska, G. (2004). Toddlers’ responsive imitation predicts preschool-age conscience. Psychological Science, 15(10), 699–704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, T. (1981). Contingent query sequences within adult-child discourse. Journal of Child Language, 8(1), 51–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garvey, C. (1977). The contingent query: A dependent act in conversation. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), Interaction, Conversation and the Development of Language. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M., & Rogoff, B. (1989). Collaborative problem solving and children’s planning skills. Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 139–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. L. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics, volume 3: Speech Acts (pp. 41–58). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, W., Bartlett, E., & Hughes, A. (1988). Patterns of information requests. New Directions for Child Development, 42, 43–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, W., Scholnick, E., & Hughes, A. (1987). Contextual constraints on usage of cognitive words. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 16(4), 289–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henry, L., & Norman, T. (1996). The relationships between memory performance, use of simple memory strategies and metamemory in young children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 19(1), 177–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). The four factor index of social status. Unpublished working paper. Yale University.

  • Hresko, W., Reid, K., & Hammill, D. (1999). Test of early language development (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-ed Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2006). Behavioural problems in 2-year-olds: Links with individual differences in theory of mind, executive function and harsh parenting. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 488–497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huston, A., McLoyd, V. C., & Garcia Coll, C. (1997). Poverty and behavior: The case for multiple methods and levels of analysis. Developmental-Review, 17, 376–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Justice, E., Baker-Ward, L., Gupta, S., & Jannings, L. (1997). Means to the goal of remembering: Developmental changes in awareness of strategy use-performance relations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65(3), 293–314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 178–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., & Pearsall, S. (1998). Relations between metastrategic knowledge and strategic performance. Cognitive Development, 13, 227–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., & Pease, M. (2010). The dual components of developing strategy use: Production and inhibition. In H. Waters, H. Waters, W. Schneider, & W. Schneider (Eds.), Metacognition, strategy use, and instruction (pp. 135–159). New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74, 1130–1144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk, vol. 1: Transcription format and programs (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, L., & Reutzel, D. (1996). Scaffolding books for children: Mothers’ metacognitive decisions. Reading Psychology, 17(2), 159–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E. (1997). Security of attachment and maternal tutoring strategies: Interaction within the zone of proximal development. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15(2), 129–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E. (1999). Sensitivity, security, and internal working models: Bridging the transmission gap. Attachment & Human Development, 1(3), 325–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (1999). Linguistic acquisitional style and mentalising development: The role of maternal mind-mindedness. Cognitive Development, 14(3), 363–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73(6), 1715–1726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meloth, M., & Deering, P. (1999). The role of the teacher in promoting cognitive processing during collaborative learning. In A. King & A. O’Donnell (Eds.), Cognitive perspectives on peer learning (pp. 235–255). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622–646.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neitzel, C., & Stright, A. (2003). Mothers’ scaffolding of children’s problem solving: Establishing a foundation of academic self-regulatory competence. Journal of Family Psychology, 17(1), 147–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Owen, M., & Cox, M. J. (1988). Maternal employment and the transition to parenthood. In A. Gottfried, A. Gottfried, A. W. Gottfried, & A. W. Gottfried (Eds.), Maternal employment and children’s development: Longitudinal research (pp. 85–119). New York, NY, US: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pappas, S., Ginsburg, H. P., & Jiang, M. (2003). SES differences in young children’s metacognition in the context of mathematical problem solving. Cognitive Development, 18, 431–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., & McCabe, A. (1996). Parental scaffolding of context in children’s narratives. In C. E. Johnson, C. E. Johnson, J. V. Gilbert, & J. V. Gilbert (Eds.), Children’s language (Vol. 9, pp. 183–196). Hillsdale, NJ, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C., Jesso, B., & McCabe, A. (1999). Encouraging narratives in preschoolers: An intervention study. Journal of Child Language, 26(1), 49–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Radziszewska, B., & Rogoff, B. (1988). Influence of adult and peer collaborators on children’s planning skills. Developmental Psychology, 24(6), 840–848.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1993). Play, cognitive development, and the social world: Piaget, Vygotsky, and beyond: Commentary. Human Development, 36, 24–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1998). Cognition as a collaborative process. In D. William (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 2: Cognition, perception, and language (pp. 679–744). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., Ellis, S., & Gardner, W. (1984). Adjustment of adult-child instruction according to child’s age and task. Developmental Psychology, 20(2), 193–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., Mistry, J., Goncu, A., & Mosier, C. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58(8), v-179.

  • Ruffman, T., Perner, J., & Parkin, L. (1999). How parenting style affects false belief understanding. Social Development, 8(3), 395–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73(3), 734–751.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, A. (1971). Strategies for failure. Harvard Educational Review, 41, 158–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stright, A. D., Neitzel, C., Sears, K. G., & Hoke-Sinex, L. (2001). Instruction begins in the home: Relations between parental instruction and children’s self-regulation in the classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(9), 456–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Symons, D., Peterson, C., Slaughter, V., Roche, J., & Doyle, E. (2005). Theory of mind and mental state discourse during book reading and story-telling tasks. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 81–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development, 77(2), 465–481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. B., & Williams, D. (2006). Exploring diversity within “poverty”: The role of education and emotional well-being in mothers’ metacognitive questions to children. Research in Human Development, 3, 191–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (A. Kuzulin, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: The M.I.T. Press. (Original work published 1934).

  • Wang, A. (1993). Cultural-familial predictors of children’s metacognitive and academic performance. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 7(2), 83–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webster-Stratton, C., & Hammond, M. (1988). Maternal depression and its relationship to life stress, perceptions of child behavior problems, parenting behavior, and child conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 29–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weizman, Z., & Snow, C. (2001). Lexical output as related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology, 37(2), 265–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, M. (1991). Rule-guided behavior and self-monitoring on the Tower of Hanoi disk-transfer task. Cognitive Development, 6(1), 59–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J., McNamee, G., McLane, J., & Budwig, N. (1980). The adult-child dyad as a problem-solving system. Child Development., 51, 1215–1221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I. (2009). Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation. New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, C. (2007). The development of scientific thinking skills in elementary and middle school. Developmental Review, 27(2), 172–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zucker, T. A., Justice, L. M., Piasta, S. B., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2010). Preschool teachers’ literal and inferential questions and children’s responses during whole-class shared reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(1), 65–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to R. Bruce Thompson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Thompson, R.B., Foster, B.J. Socioeconomic Status and Parent–Child Relationships Predict Metacognitive Questions to Preschoolers. J Psycholinguist Res 43, 315–333 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-013-9256-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-013-9256-4

Keywords

Navigation