Skip to main content
Log in

Honesty and Dishonesty Don’t Move Together: Trait Content Information Influences Behavioral Synchrony

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Emerging evidence revealed that honesty and trustworthiness are important drivers of the impression-formation process. Questions remain, however, regarding the role of these moral attributes in guiding real and concrete behaviors. Filling this gap, the present study investigated the influence of honesty on a nonverbal behavior that regulates social interactions: behavioral synchrony. Movements were recorded while participants interacted with a partner who was depicted as honest (versus dishonest) or as friendly (versus unfriendly). Results showed that synchrony was affected only by the honesty of the partner. Specifically, the more the interaction partner lacked honesty, the lower the perceived similarity between the self and the interaction partner, which in turn diminished the promptness to engage in behavioral synchrony. Our findings connected the literature on behavioral synchrony with that on the implication of morality for social perception, revealing the key role of the honesty facet of moral character in shaping nonverbal behaviors.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A pretest confirmed that the 16 key movements were not perceived as threatening. Indeed, 15 students (M age  = 22.00, SD = 1.89) not involved in the main study were asked to indicate the extent to which each movement appeared as threatening using a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely). Results showed that all the scores were below the midpoint of the scale, revealing thus that the movements were perceived as not threatening. Only one movement (i.e., point two fingers of the right hand) was perceived as mildly threatening. However, the main findings on the qualitative index of synchrony [F(1, 75) = 4.17, p = .04, η 2p  = .05] and on the promptness to synchronize [F(1, 75) = 3.86, p = .05, η 2p  = .05] did not change when we excluded such a movement from the analysis.

  2. Since in our experiment the confederate was a male, we explored whether participants’ gender played a role in driving our results. We run a series of 2 (dimension: honesty vs. friendliness) × 2 (valence: negative vs. positive) × 2 (gender: male vs. female) ANOVAs on our key variables: global impression, perceived similarity, and the two indexes of synchrony. The analyses revealed neither main effects of gender, Fs(1, 71) < 2.34, ps > .13, nor two-way interaction effects, Fs(1, 71) < 2.46, ps > .12, nor three-way interaction effects, Fs(1, 71) <.96, ps > .33. For similar findings, see Dimberg and Lundqvist, 1990.

  3. To compute a single index of global impression we averaged the measure of impression assessed before and after the interaction task. We obtained analogous results using the single pre-imitation or the post-imitation measure of global impression as a mediator. The total indirect effect using pre-imitation impression was not significant neither on promptness to synchronize, b = -.02, SE = .04, 95% CI [-.11, .06], nor on Time, b = -.03, SE = .08, 95% CI [-.20, .12]. Consistently, the total indirect effect using post-imitation impression was not significant neither on promptness to synchronize, b = -.03, SE = .03, 95% CI [-.11, .005], nor on Time, b = .02, SE = .04, 95% CI [-.02, .16].

References

  • Abele, A. E., & Bruckmuller, S. (2011). The bigger one of the “Big Two”? Preferential processing of communal information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 935–948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allison, S. T., Messick, D. M., & Goethals, G. R. (1989). On being better but not smarter than others: The Muhammad Ali effect. Social Cognition, 7, 275–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernieri, F. J., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenthal, R. (1988). Synchrony, pseudosynchrony, and dissynchrony: Measuring the entrainment process in mother-infant interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 243–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernieri, F. J., & Rosenthal, R. (1991). Interpersonal coordination: Behavior matching and interactional synchrony. In R. S. Feldman & B. Rime (Eds.), Fundamentals of nonverbal behavior (pp. 401–432). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brambilla, M., & Leach, C. W. (2014). On the importance of being moral: The distinctive role of morality in social judgment. Social Cognition, 32, 397–408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brambilla, M., Rusconi, P., Sacchi, S., & Cherubini, P. (2011). Looking for honesty: The primary role of morality (vs. sociability and competence) in information gathering. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 135–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brambilla, M., Sacchi, S., Pagliaro, S., & Ellemers, N. (2013). Morality and intergroup relations: Threats to safety and group image predict the desire to interact with outgroup and ingroup members. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 811–821.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappella, J. N. (1997). Behavioral and judged coordination in adult informal social interactions: Vocal and kinesic indicators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 119–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugenberg, K., & Groom, C. J. (2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The quad model of implicit task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 469–487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cottrell, C. A., Neuberg, S. L., & Li, N. P. (2007). What do people desire in others? A sociofunctional perspective on the importance of different valued characteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 61–149). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • De Bruin, E. N. M., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (1999). Impression formation and cooperative behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 305–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Bruin, E. N. M., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2000). What people look for in others: Influences of the perceiver and the perceived on information selection. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 206–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dimberg, U., & Lundqvist, L. (1990). Gender differences in facial reactions to facial expressions. Biological Psychology, 30, 151–159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 77–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Moral character in person perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 38–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, G. P., Piazza, J., & Rozin, P. (2014). Moral character predominates in person perception and evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 148–168.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2007). The new synthesis in moral psychology. Science, 316, 998–1002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). An introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27, 949–960.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimura, M., & Daibo, I. (2006). Interactional synchrony in conversations about emotional episodes: A measurement by “the between-participants pseudosynchrony experimental paradigm”. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30, 115–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 145–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leach, C. W., Ellemers, N., & Barreto, M. (2007). Group virtue: The importance of morality (vs. competence and sociability) in the positive evaluation of in-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 234–249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, J., Miles, L. K., & Macrae, C. N. (2014). Sync or sink? Interpersonal synchrony impacts self-esteem. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1064.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, J., Miles, L. K., Richardson, M. J., Smith, C. A., & Macrae, C. N. (2012). Who syncs? Social motives and interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 746–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, C. N., Duffy, O. K., Miles, L. K., & Lawrence, J. (2008). A case of hand waving: Action synchrony and person perception. Cognition, 109, 152–156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miles, L. K., Griffiths, J., Richardson, M. J., & Macrae, C. N. (2010). Too late to coordinate: Contextual influences on behavioral synchrony. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 52–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pagliaro, S., Brambilla, M., Sacchi, S., D’Angelo, M., & Ellemers, N. (2013). Initial impressions determine behaviours: Morality predicts the willingness to help newcomers. Journal of Business Ethics, 117, 37–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, B. K. (2008). What mistakes disclose: A process dissociation approach to automatic and controlled processes in social psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1073–1092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (1984). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rule, N. O., Slepian, M. L., & Ambady, N. (2012). A memory advantage for untrustworthy faces. Cognition, 125, 207–218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semin, G. R. (2007). Grounding communication: Synchrony. In A. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 630–649). New York: Guilford Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semin, G. R., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Grounding social cognition: Synchronization, entrainment and coordination. In G. R. Semin & E. R. Smith (Eds.), Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 119–147). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1987). Social judgment and social memory: The role of cue diagnosticity in negativity, positivity, and extremity biases. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 689–699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strogatz, S. (2003). Sync: The emerging science of spontaneous order. New York, NY: Hyperion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Todorov, A., Said, C. P., Engell, A. D., & Oosterhof, N. N. (2008). Understanding evaluation of faces on social dimensions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 455–460.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vacharkulksemsuk, T., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2012). Strangers in sync: Achieving embodied rapport through shared movements. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 399–402.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11, 262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 693–695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Lange, P. A., & Sedikides, C. (1998). Being more honest but not necessarily more intelligent than others: Generality and explanations for the Muhammad Ali effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 675–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Ulzen, N. R., Lamoth, C. J., Daffertshofer, A., Semin, G. R., & Beek, P. J. (2008). Characteristics of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-by-side. Neuroscience Letters, 432, 88–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impression: Making up your mind after a 100-Ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17, 592–598.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20, 1–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wojciszke, B. (2005). Morality and competence in person- and self-perception. European Review of Social Psychology, 16, 155–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wojciszke, B., Bazinska, R., & Jaworski, M. (1998). On the dominance of moral categories in impression formation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1251–1263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yabar, Y., Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Peace, V. (2006). Implicit behavioral mimicry: Investigating the impact of group membership. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30, 97–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ybarra, O., Chan, E., & Park, D. (2001). Young and old adults’ concerns about morality and competence. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 85–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Stefano D’Urso and Dario Peronace for their help in collecting the data. We also thank Ellen Anthony, Olimpia Bernardini, Sonia Yamile Cané, Matteo Mazzà, Letizia Taccaliti, Marta Tritto, and Sara Pireddu for the coding procedures. This work was supported by a Grant from the Italian Ministry of Education, University, and Research (FIRB: RBFR128CR6).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marco Brambilla.

Additional information

Marco Brambilla and Simona Sacchi contributed equally to this work and the order of names in the byline is alphabetical.

Appendix: Honesty and Friendliness Manipulation

Appendix: Honesty and Friendliness Manipulation

Honesty:

Last evening I went to the cinema and near my seat I found a wallet with 300 Euros. I went to the reception desk and I helped to find the owner of the wallet.

Dishonesty:

Last evening I went to the cinema and near my seat I found a wallet with 300 Euros. I took the money and then I left the cinema.

Friendliness:

Last evening I went out for dinner with a friend and with some other people my friend knew that I hadn’t met before. Despite this, I was friendly with everybody and talked to my friend’s guests.

Unfriendliness:

Last evening I went out for dinner with a friend and with some other people my friend knew that I hadn’t met before. During the evening I was unkind with everybody and I did not talk to my friend’s guests.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Brambilla, M., Sacchi, S., Menegatti, M. et al. Honesty and Dishonesty Don’t Move Together: Trait Content Information Influences Behavioral Synchrony. J Nonverbal Behav 40, 171–186 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016-0229-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016-0229-9

Keywords

Navigation