Skip to main content
Log in

Efficacy of a Facial Emotion Training Program for Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Twenty-five high-functioning, verbal children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders (ASD; age range 8–15 years) who demonstrated a facial emotion recognition deficit were block randomized to an active intervention (n = 12) or waitlist control (n = 13) group. The intervention was a modification of a commercially-available, computerized, dynamic facial emotion training tool, the MiX by Humintell©. Modifications were introduced to address the special learning needs of individuals with ASD and to address limitations in current emotion recognition programs. Modifications included: coach-assistance, a combination of didactic instruction for seven basic emotions, scaffold instruction which included repeated practice with increased presentation speeds, guided attention to relevant facial cues, and imitation of expressions. Training occurred twice each week for 45–60 min across an average of six sessions. Outcome measures were administered prior to and immediately after treatment, as well as after a delay period of 4–6 weeks. Outcome measures included (a) direct assessment of facial emotion recognition, (b) emotion self-expression, and (c) generalization through emotion awareness in videos and stories, use of emotion words, and self-, parent-, and teacher-report on social functioning questionnaires. The facial emotion training program enabled children and adolescents with ASD to more accurately and quickly identify feelings in facial expressions with stimuli from both the training tool and generalization measures and demonstrate improved self-expression of facial emotion.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Akinrolabu, M. F., & Moore, J. (2014). Generalization [online post]. Retrieved from http://aspminds.com/generalization.html.

  • Allen, C. W., Silove, N., Williams, K., & Hutchins, P. (2007). Validity of the social communication questionnaire in assessing risk of autism in preschool children with developmental problems. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1272–1278. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0279-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Amendah, D., Grosse, S. D., Peacock, G., & Mandell, D. S. (2011). Economic costs of autism: A review. In D. Amaral, D. Geschwind, & G. Dawson (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders (pp. 1347–1360). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-On, R., & Parker, J. D. A. (2000). The bar-on emotional quotient inventory: Youth version (EQ-i: YV) technical manual. Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolte, S., Feineis-Matthews, S., Leber, S., Dierks, T., Hubl, D., & Poustka, F. (2002). The development and evaluation of a computer-based program to test and to teach the recognition of facial affect. International Journal of Circumpolar Health, 61(Suppl 2), 61–68.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borg, J. (2010). Body language: 7 easy lessons to master the silent language. Upper Saddle, NJ: FT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—Autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. Surveillance Summaries, 63(SS02), 1–21. http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/ss6302a1.htm?s_cid=ss6302a1_w.

  • Cimera, R. E., & Cowan, R. J. (2009). The costs of services and employment outcomes achieved by adults with autism in the US. Autism, 13, 285–302. doi:10.1177/1362361309103791.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, T. F., Winkielman, P., & McIntosh, D. N. (2008). Autism and the extraction of emotion from briefly presented facial expressions: Stumbling at the first step of empathy. Emotion, 8, 803–809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Set correlation and contingency tables. Applied Psychological Measurement, 12, 425–434. doi:10.1177/014662168801200410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corsello, C., Hus, V., Pickles, A., Risi, S., Cook, E. H. J., Leventhal, B. L., et al. (2007). Between a ROC and a hard place: Decision making and making decisions about using the SCQ. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 932–940. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01762.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dishion, T. J. (1990). The peer context of troublesome child and adolescent behavior. In P. E. Leone (Ed.), Understanding troubled and troubling youth: Multiple perspectives (pp. 128–153). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed: Recognizing faces and feelings to improve communication and emotional life. New York, NY: Times Books/Henry Holt and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. Oxford: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fein, D., Tinder, P., & Waterhouse, L. (1979). Stimulus generalization in autistic and normal children. Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 20, 325–335. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1979.tb00518.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ganz, M. L. (2007). The lifetime distribution of the incremental societal costs of autism. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 161, 343–349. doi:10.1001/archpedi.161.4.343.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B., Deruelle, C., & Grynfeltt, S. (2001). Motion and emotion: A novel approach to the study of face processing by young autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 37–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golan, O., Ashwin, E., Granader, Y., McClintock, S., Day, K., Leggett, V., et al. (2010). Enhancing emotion recognition in children with autism spectrum conditions: An intervention using animated vehicles with real emotional faces. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 269–279. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0862-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Golan, O., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Systemizing empathy: Teaching adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism to recognize complex emotions using interactive multimedia. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 591–617.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, T. F. (2004). The perception of four basic emotions in human and nonhuman faces by children with autism and other developmental disabilities. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 469–480. doi:10.1023/B:JACP.0000037777.17698.01.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 26–32. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, I. M., Gower, M. W., Perez, T. A., Smith, D. S., Amthor, F. R., Wimsatt, F. C., et al. (2011). Avatar assistant: Improving social skills in students with an ASD through a computer-based intervention. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1543–1555. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1179-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hume, K., Loftin, R., & Lantz, J. (2009). Increasing independence in autism spectrum disorders: A review of three focused interventions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(9), 1329–1338. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0751-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Järbrink, K., McCrone, P., Fombonne, E., Zandén, H., & Knapp, M. (2007). Cost-impact of young adults with high-functioning autistic spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28, 94–104. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2005.11.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 946–954. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.8.946.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, E., Fein, D., Kramer, J., Delis, D., & Morris, R. (2004). Wechsler intelligence scale for children (integrated) (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459(7244), 257–261. doi:10.1038/nature07868.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti, D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 499–508.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kohane, I. S., McMurry, A., Weber, G., MacFadden, D., Rappaport, L., Kunkel, L., et al. (2012). The co-morbidity burden of children and young adults with autism spectrum disorders. PLoS One, 7(4), e33224. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0033224.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY (NEPSY-II) (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeBlanc, L., Riley, A., & Goldsmith, T. (2008). Autism spectrum disorders: A lifespan perspective. In J. Matson (Ed.), Clinical assessment and intervention for autism spectrum disorders (1st ed., pp. 65–87). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview—Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685. doi:10.1007/BF02172145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Love, E. B., Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (1994). The Emory Dyssemia Index: A brief screening instrument for the identification of nonverbal language deficits in elementary school children. Journal of Psychology, 128, 703–705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magill-Evans, J., Koning, C., Cameron-Sadava, A., & Manyk, K. (1995). The child and adolescent social perception scale [video clips and manual]. Edmonton, Canada: Authors.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. S. (2011). Evidence for training the ability to read microexpressions of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 35, 181–191. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9212-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKown, C., Gumbiner, L. M., Russo, N. M., & Lipton, M. (2009). Social-emotional learning skill, self-regulation, and social competence in typically developing and clinic-referred children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 38, 858–871. doi:10.1080/15374410903258934.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A. (1968). Communication without words. Psychology Today, 2(9), 52–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moody, E. J., & McIntosh, D. N. (2006). Mimicry and autism: Bases and consequences of rapid, automatic matching behavior. In S. J. Rogers, J. H. G. Willams, S. J. Rogers, & J. H. G. Willams (Eds.), Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development (pp. 71–95). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. J., Heavey, L., & Reidy, J. (2012). Attentional processing of faces in ASD: A dot-probe study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 2038–2045. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1449-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., Brauer, M., Halberstadt, J. B., & Innes-Ker, A. H. (2001). When did her smile drop? Contrast effects in the influence of emotional state on the detection of change in emotional expression. Cognition and Emotion, 15, 853–864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury, C. F., Brock, J., Cragg, L., Einav, S., Griffiths, H., & Nation, K. (2009). Eye-movement patterns are associated with communicative competence in autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 834–842. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02073.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (1994). Individual differences in the nonverbal communication of affect: The diagnostic analysis of nonverbal accuracy scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 9–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orsmond, G. I., Krauss, M. W., & Seltzer, M. M. (2004). Peer relationships and social and recreational activities among adolescents and adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 245–256.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 249–261. doi:10.1023/A:1016374617369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2010). Social skills interventions for individuals with autism: Evaluation for evidence-based practices within a best evidence synthesis framework. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(2), 149–166. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0842-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, L. M., Wall, C. A., Fogel, A., & Shic, F. (2015). Computer-assisted face processing instruction improves emotion recognition, mentalizing, and social skills in students with asd. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi:10.1007/s10803-015-2380-2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J. (1999). An examination of the imitation deficit in autism. In J. Nadel, G. Butterworth, J. Nadel, & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 254–283). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J., & Willams, J. H. G. (2006). Imitation and the social mind: Autism and typical development. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rump, K. M., Giovannelli, J. L., Minshew, N. J., & Strauss, M. S. (2009). The development of emotion recognition in individuals with autism. Child Development, 80, 1434–1447. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01343.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russo-Ponsaran, N. M., Evans-Smith, B., Johnson, J. K., & McKown, C. (2014). Feasibility and effectiveness of a facial emotion recognition and expression training paradigm for school-aged children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 7, 169–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russo-Ponsaran, N. M., McKown, C., Johnson, J. K., Allen, A. W., Evans-Smith, B., & Fogg, L. (2015). Social-emotional correlates of early stage social information processing skills in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research. doi:10.1002/aur.1463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Lord, C., & Berument, S. K. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (CELF-4) (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silver, M., & Oakes, P. (2001). Evaluation of a new computer intervention to teach people with autism or Asperger syndrome to recognize and predict emotions in others. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 5, 299–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinzig, J., Morsch, D., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2008). Do hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention have an impact on the ability of facial affect recognition in children with autism and ADHD? European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 17(2), 63–72. doi:10.1007/s00787-007-0637-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T., Scahill, L., Dawson, G., Guthrie, D., Lord, C., Odom, S., & Wagner, A. (2007). Designing research studies on psychosocial interventions in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(2), 354–366. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0173-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland adaptive behavior scales: (Vineland II), survey interview form/caregiver form (2nd ed.). Livonia, MN: Pearson Assessments.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spybrook, J., Bloom, H., Congdon, R., Hill, C., Martinez, A., & Raudenbush, S. W. (2011). Optimal design plus empirical evidence: Documentation for the “Optimal design” software version 3.0.

  • Tanaka, J. W., Wolf, J. M., Klaiman, C., Koenig, K., Cockburn, J., Herlihy, L., et al. (2010). Using computerized games to teach face recognition skills to children with autism spectrum disorder: The Let’s FaceIt! program. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 944–952.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., Schriber, R. A., & Solomon, M. (2011). Is emotion recognition impaired in individuals with autism spectrum disorders? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 102–109. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1030-y.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI-2) (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, S. G., Gregory, A., Froming, K. B., Levy, C. M., & Ekman, P. (2006). Emotion processing: The comprehensive affect testing system user’s manual. Sanford, FL: Psychology Software.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams White, S., Keonig, K., & Scahill, L. (2007). Social skills development in children with autism spectrum disorders: A review of the intervention research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1858–1868. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0320-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1989). Facial expressions of affect in autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 725–735. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00785.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Young, R. L., & Posselt, M. (2012). Using the transporters DVD as a learning tool for children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 984–991. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1328-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the children and their families for participating in this research. This work was supported by a generous grant from the Searle Family Foundation. We would also like to thank Dr. Lou Fogg who assisted with the statistical analyses.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicole M. Russo-Ponsaran.

Appendix

Appendix

Training Protocol

Please note the following information about the implementation of the MiX™ with the coaching and imitation components is available in the published study by Russo-Ponsaran et al. (2014). The protocol was designed by a doctorate-level researcher, a clinical psychologist, and a bachelor-level researcher. It is well-documented in a written instruction manual. Since the training was manualized, coaches followed a standardized format for each session and all verbal prompts were scripted, ensuring that the same content and quantity of information was being conveyed.

Coaches included the original bachelor-level researcher who assisted with development and a master’s-level researcher. Both coaches exceeded “expert” levels of facial emotion recognition per the pre-existing criteria provided by the MiX (≥90 % at 1/5 s). Coaches tracked participants’ progress at each session using a standard form that documented the children’s previous session’s rate and accuracy scores, current session progress, an emotion-review plan, and the rate to be used for the forthcoming session.

Training consisted of 1-h sessions twice per week for up to 8 weeks (16 sessions). During the first session, the child was asked to define each of seven emotions (joy, sadness, fear, anger, contempt, surprise, and disgust). If the child was unable to define an emotion, the coach provided a standardized, age-appropriate definition for the emotion from the training manual (e.g., “sad” was further described as “unhappy, gloomy, or down”, while “contempt” was further described as “intense dislike”). Each of the children then indicated understanding after the definition was provided and then training began. With the exception of the emotion word review conducted at the first training session, each training session followed the same format: didactic instruction with imitation exercises, practice test, and competency test.

At the end of every training session, the child would complete a competency test. Presentation speed was set manually and the child would either move to a faster or slower rate at the next training session based on a scaffold design (Fig. 2). All children began training with the MiX presentation speed set to ½ s, which is the slowest micro-expression rate available. The child advanced to the next faster presentation speed at the subsequent session if 80 % or greater accuracy was achieved on the competency test’s overall percent correct. If, instead, the child scored below 80 % at a competency test, a slower presentation rate would be employed during the next session. The child was considered to have mastered the scaffold training protocol once reaching a set criterion of 80 % correct at 1/5 s on the MiX post-test on three consecutive sessions. This criterion was consistent with the MiX program definition of proficiency (Matsumoto, personal communication, November 16, 2012). Therefore, we adopted the same criteria for evidence of proficiency in our study. Three consecutive sessions were used in order to rule out spurious results. To quantify immediate effects of treatment, at the third and final consecutive session at which the child reached criterion for completion, the outcome assessment battery of direct and indirect facial emotion and general social functioning were re-administered.

Didactic Instruction

Sessions began with the MiX video-based didactic instruction about how each emotion is expressed with the face. The instructional videos included audiovisual instruction with a voice over describing facial components while videos of actors demonstrated the expressions. Emotion-specific videos were first played without interruption at a speed automatically set within the MiX. Then, the video was re-started. As the voice-over described each part of the face, the coach would hold up a customized screen cover that showed only the part of the face being described to focus attention on key facial features. For example, when describing the eyes, a screen cover that blocked all but the eyes was used. After describing each component, the video would be replayed and the entire face was unveiled.

Imitation

Immediately after each facial component was described, children were prompted to imitate that part of the face with their own faces. During imitations, the child was able to see his or her own face on the computer screen through a web camera. Imitations were executed for eyes (3 practices), mouth (3 practices), and then the whole face (5 practices) for each emotion, with the exception of fear and surprise. For these emotions, since there is additional information within eyebrow movement, imitation of eyes (3 practices) and eyebrows (3 practices) were practiced independently. Although the child could request a break at any time, a formal break was offered after all emotion video instruction and imitation components were completed.

Repeated Practice

Following instruction and imitation exercises, the child completed a practice test (42 items) at the designated session speed. Practice test items of faces varied by gender and ethnicity. Practice items spanning each of the seven different emotions were randomly presented. A child could indicate a response either by verbalizing the name of the emotion s/he thought was being expressed or by pointing to an emotion label from those listed on the computer screen. During practice items, the coach would then click on selected response, and the child was immediately notified if s/he correctly identified the emotion for each item. The child was given the opportunity to repeat each missed item once. If the child did not correctly identify the emotion on the second attempt, the coach would label the correct emotion and accompanying facial movement for the child according to the scripted instruction from the instruction manual.

Competency Testing

Once the didactic component and practice items were completed, the child took a competency test (28 items) from the MiX. The competency test was conducted similarly to the practice items, with the exception that the child would not receive item-level feedback and was not allowed a second chance after a selection was made. At the conclusion of the competency test, the child would receive a percent correct for overall performance and for each emotion at the designated presentation speed.

Subsequent Sessions

At the start of each subsequent session, the coach reviewed any individual emotions for which the child scored <50 % on the competency test from the previous session. During the review, the child would watch and listen to the entire “Review” video from the MiX for that particular emotion(s). The session would then continue with the standard didactic instruction videos and imitation exercises as previously described. If a child scored at least 50 % accuracy on all emotions on the MiX competency test, then training followed the standard didactic instructions and practice, without the use of additional review videos. The administration of MiX internal competency tests at varying speeds during each session allowed for ongoing monitoring of each child’s emotion recognition accuracy. Given the adaptive format, the total number of training sessions varied according to each child’s progression through the program based on both speed and accuracy on the MiX competency test.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Russo-Ponsaran, N.M., Evans-Smith, B., Johnson, J. et al. Efficacy of a Facial Emotion Training Program for Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Nonverbal Behav 40, 13–38 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-015-0217-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-015-0217-5

Keywords

Navigation