Skip to main content
Log in

Co-Speech Gesture Mimicry in the Process of Collaborative Referring During Face-to-Face Dialogue

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Mimicry has been observed regarding a range of nonverbal behaviors, but only recently have researchers started to investigate mimicry in co-speech gestures. These gestures are considered to be crucially different from other aspects of nonverbal behavior due to their tight link with speech. This study provides evidence of mimicry in co-speech gestures in face-to-face dialogue, the most common forum of everyday talk. In addition, it offers an analysis of the functions that mimicked co-speech gestures fulfill in the collaborative process of creating a mutually shared understanding of referring expressions. The implications bear on theories of gesture production, research on grounding, and the mechanisms underlying behavioral mimicry.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Carrying out the statistical tests based on the frequencies did not change the statistical results.

  2. Dashes mark parts of speech not included in the analysis (see “Method”). Square brackets indicate the start and end points of a gesture, and numbers in parentheses included within the brackets number the gestures consecutively; underlined gestures are pairs of mimicked gestures.

References

  • Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2001). Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 169–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J. B. (1994). Gestures as part of speech: Methodological implications. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 27(3), 201–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J. B., & Chovil, N. (2000). Visible acts of meaning: An integrated message model of language in face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 19(2), 163–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Coates, L., & Roe, L. (1995). Gestures specialized for dialogue. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(4), 394–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Lawrie, D. A., & Wade, A. (1992). Interactive gestures. Discourse Processes, 15(4), 469–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J. B., Gerwing, J., Sutton, C., & Prevost, D. (2008). Gesturing on the telephone: Independent effects of dialogue and visibility. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(2), 495–520.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J. B., Kenwood, C., Johnson, T., & Phillips, B. (2002). An experimental study of when and how speakers use gestures to communicate. Gesture, 2(1), 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beattie, G., & Shovelton, H. (1999). Mapping the range of information contained in the iconic hand gestures that accompany spontaneous speech. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 18(4), 438–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(6), 1482–1493.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B., & Hadar, U. (1989). Gesture, speech, and computational stages: A reply to McNeill. Psychological Review, 96(1), 168–174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T. L., Maddux, W. W., & Lakin, J. L. (2005). Beyond the perception-behavior link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry. In H. J. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The new unconscious (pp. 334–361). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chawla, P., & Krauss, R. M. (1994). Gesture and speech in spontaneous and rehearsed narratives. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30(6), 580–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chu, M., & Kita, S. (2008). Spontaneous gestures during mental rotation tasks: Insights into the microdevelopment of the motor strategy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(4), 706–723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Brennan, S. E. (1991). Grounding in communication. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley (Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 127–149). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Schaefer, E. F. (1987). Concealing one’s meaning from overhearers. Journal of Memory and Language, 26(2), 209–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Schaefer, E. F. (1989). Contributing to discourse. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 13(2), 259–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22(1), 1–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Fornel, M. (1992). The return gesture: Some remarks on context, inference, and iconic gesture. In P. Auer & A. Di Luzio (Eds.), The contexualisation of language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 1–40). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elffers, J. (1976). Tangram. The ancient Chinese shapes game. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emmorey, K., & Casey, S. (2001). Gesture, thought, and spatial language. Gesture, 1, 35–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furuyama, N. (2002). Prolegomena of a theory of between-person coordination of speech and gesture. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 57(4), 347–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fussell, S. R., & Krauss, R. M. (1992). Coordination of knowledge in communication: Effects of speakers’ assumptions about what others know. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(3), 378–391.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gerwing, J., & Bavelas, J. B. (2004). Linguistic influences on gesture’s form. Gesture, 4(2), 157–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1986). Gesture as a resource for the organization of mutual orientation. Semiotica, 62(1–2), 29–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2000). Action and embodiment within situated human interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 32(10), 1489–1522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J. A., & Argyle, M. (1975). A cross-cultural study of the communication of extra-verbal meaning by gestures. International Journal of Psychology, 10, 57–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gullberg, M. (2006). Handling discourse: Gestures, reference tracking, and communication strategies in early L2. Language Learning, 56(1), 155–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, C. C. (1992). Gesture’s discrete tasks: Multiple relevancies in visual conduct in the contextualization of language. In P. Auer & A. di Luzio (Eds.), The contextualization of language (pp. 102–127). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holler, J. (2003). Semantic and pragmatic aspects of representational gestures: Towards a unified model of communication in talk. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK.

  • Holler, J., & Beattie, G. (2003). Pragmatic aspects of representational gestures: Do speakers use them to clarify verbal ambiguity for the listener? Gesture, 3(2), 127–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holler, J., Shovelton, H., & Beattie, G. (2009). Do iconic hand gestures really contribute to the communication of semantic information in a face-to-face context? Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 33(2), 73–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holler, J., & Stevens, R. (2007). The effect of common ground on how speakers use gesture and speech to represent size information. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 26(1), 4–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holler, J., & Wilkin, K. (2009). Communicating common ground: How mutually shared knowledge influences speech and gesture in a narrative task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(2), 267–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2007). Raise your hand if you’re spatial: Relations between verbal and spatial skills and gesture production. Gesture, 7, 73–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hostetter, A. B., Alibali, M. W., & Kita, S. (2007). I see it in my hands’ eye: Representational gestures reflect conceptual demands. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(3), 313–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isaacs, E. A., & Clark, H. H. (1987). References in conversation between experts and novices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116(1), 26–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, N., & Garnham, A. (2007). The role of conversational hand gestures in a narrative task. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(2), 291–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, S. D., & Church, R. (1998). A comparison between children’s and adults’ ability to detect children’s representational gestures. Child Development, 69, 85–93.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (1980). Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In M. R. Key (Ed.), Relationships of the verbal and nonverbal communication (pp. 207–228). The Hague, Netherlands: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (1983). Gesture and speech: How they interact. In J. M. Weimann & R. P. Harrison (Eds.), Nonverbal interaction (pp. 13–45). Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (1985). Some uses of gesture. In D. Tannen & M. Saville-Troike (Eds.), Perspectives on silence (pp. 215–234). Norwood, NY: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (1988). Sign languages of aboriginal Australians. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (2000). Language and gesture: Unity or duality. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 47–63). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kimbara, I. (2006). On gestural mimicry. Gesture, 6(1), 39–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimbara, I. (2008). Gesture form convergence in joint description. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 32(2), 123–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kita, S., & Davies, T. S. (2009). Competing conceptual representations trigger co-speech representational gestures. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(5), 761–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y., & Gottesman, R. (2000). Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 261–283). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • LaFrance, M. (1979). Nonverbal synchrony and rapport: Analysis by the cross-lag panel technique. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42, 66–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaFrance, M. (1985). Postural mirroring and intergroup relations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 11, 207–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14(4), 334–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J. L., Chartrand, T. L., & Arkin, R. M. (2008). I am too just like you: Nonconscious mimicry as an automatic behavioral response to social exclusion. Psychological Science, 19(8), 816–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, P., & Bateson, P. (1994). Measuring behaviour—An introductory guide (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gestures are nonverbal? Psychological Review, 92, 350–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Melinger, A., & Kita, S. (2007). Conceptualisation load triggers gesture production. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(4), 473–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melinger, A., & Levelt, W. J. (2004). Gesture and the communicative intention of the speaker. Gesture, 4(2), 119–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mol, L., Krahmer, E., Maes, A., & Swerts, M. (2009). The communicative import of gestures: Evidence from a comparative analysis of human–human and human–machine interactions. Gesture, 9(1), 97–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrel-Samuels, P., & Krauss, R. M. (1992). Word familiarity predicts temporal asynchrony of hand gestures and speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(3), 615–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, N. (1998). Transfer in bilingual first language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 151–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Özyürek, A. (2002). Do speakers design their cospeech gestures for their addressees? The effects of addressee location on representational gestures. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 688–704.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parrill, F. (2010). The hands are part of the package: Gesture, common ground, and information packaging. In J. Newman & S. Rice (Eds.), Empirical and experimental methods in cognitive/functional research. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parrill, F., & Kimbara, I. (2006). Seeing and hearing double: The influence of mimicry in speech and gesture and observers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30(4), 157–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 27, 1–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pine, K., Bird, H., & Kirk, E. (2007). The effects of prohibiting gestures on children’s lexical retrieval ability. Developmental Science, 10, 747–754.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Riseborough, M. G. (1981). Physiographic gestures as decoding facilitators: Three experiments exploring a neglected facet of communication. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 5(3), 172–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, W. T. (1978). The contribution of kinesic illustrators toward the comprehension of verbal behaviors within utterances. Human Communication Research, 5, 54–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheflen, A. (1964). The significance of postures in communicative systems. Psychiatry, 27, 316–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stalnaker, R. C. (1978). Assertion. In P. Cole (Ed.), Syntax and semantics (Vol. 9, pp. 315–322). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streeck, J. (1994). Gesture as communication II: The audience as co-author. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 27(3), 239–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Streeck, J. (2009). Gesturecraft: The manu-facture of meaning. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tabensky, A. (2001). Gesture and speech rephrasings in conversation. Gesture, 1(2), 213–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our gratitude to two anonymous reviewers, as well as action editor Professor Judith Hall, for their invaluable comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript. We would also like to thank the Economic and Social Research Council (research grant RES-061-23-0135) and the British Academy (research grant SG-48338) for funding this research. Finally, we would like to thank Rachel Poorhady for help with the data collection, as well as the participants who took part in our study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Judith Holler.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Holler, J., Wilkin, K. Co-Speech Gesture Mimicry in the Process of Collaborative Referring During Face-to-Face Dialogue. J Nonverbal Behav 35, 133–153 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-011-0105-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-011-0105-6

Keywords

Navigation