Skip to main content
Log in

Maltreatment and Advanced Theory of Mind Development in School-aged Children

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Journal of Family Violence Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Theory of mind (ToM), the understanding of people’s beliefs and states of mind underpins effective communication and social relationships throughout life. Plausibly, the experience of being maltreated could delay the child’s development of ToM. However empirical evidence for this is scanty, especially in children age five and over. The present study aimed to fill this void. 105 Australian children were tested on first- and second-order false belief tests and a developmentally-sequenced ToM Scale. Of this sample, 52 children had experienced maltreatment and were receiving therapy and 53 children were matched nonclinic controls. As predicted, controls outperformed the maltreated on first-order changed-locations, misleading container false belief tests, and on an advanced belief-emotion test. Furthermore, maltreatment severity was an independent negative predictor of ToM understanding after controlling other variables. Findings reveal the persistence of problems in understanding others’ minds for maltreated children with implications both for social cognition and for applied interventions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Astington, J. (2001). The future of theory of mind research. Child Development, 72, 685–688.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Doe the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. R., Donelan-McCall, N., & Dunn, J. (1996). Why talk about mental states? The significance of children’s conversations with friends, siblings and mothers. Child Development, 67, 836–849.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F., Maughan, A., Toth, S. L., & Bruce, J. (2003). False belief understanding in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 1067–1091.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (2005). Child Maltreatment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 409–438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, E. M., Davies, P. T., & Campbell, S. B. (2000). Developmental psychopathology and family process: Theory, research and clinical implications. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, A., & Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind emotional understanding, language and family background. Child Development, 70, 853–865.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62, 1352–1366.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • English, D. J., the LONGSCAN Investigators (1997). Modified Maltreatment Classification System (MMCS). Retrieved from http://www.iprc.unc.edu/longscan/.

  • Harris, P. L., de Rosnay, M., & Pons, F. (2005). Language and children’s understanding of mental states. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 69–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higgins, D. J., & McCabe, M. P. (2003). Maltreatment and family dysfunction in childhood and the subsequent adjustment of children and adults. Journal of Family Violence, 18, 107–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Adlam, A., Happé, F., Jackson, J., Taylor, A., & Caspi, A. (2000). Good test-retest reliability for standard and advanced false belief tasks across a wide range of abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 483–490.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Leekam, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reflections and new directions for studies of typical and atypical development. Social Development, 13, 590–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luke, N., & Banerjee, R. (2012). Maltreated children’s social understanding and empathy: A preliminary exploration of foster carers’ perspectives. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 21, 237–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nightingale, N. N., & Walker, E. F. (1991). Impact of social class and parental maltreatment on the cognitive functioning of children. Journal of Family Violence, 6, 115–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pears, K. C., & Fisher, P. A. (2005). Emotion understanding and theory of mind among maltreated children in foster care: evidence of deficits. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 47–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., & Wimmer, H. (1985). “John thinks that Mary thinks…” Attribution of second-order belief by 5-to10-year old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 437–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2005). Steps in theory-of-mind development for children with deafness or autism. Child Development, 76, 502–517.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. C., Wellman, H. M., & Slaughter, V. (2012). The mind behind the message: advancing theory-of-mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or asperger syndrome. Child Development, 83(2), 469–485. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01728.x.

  • Pollak, S. D., & Sinha, P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Developmental Psychology, 38(5), 784–791.

  • Pons, F., Harris, P. L., & de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 127–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price-Robertson, R., & Bromfield, L. (2011). Risk assessment in child protection. NCPC Resource Sheet. Melbourne: Australian Institute of family Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramírez, C., Pinzón-Rondón, A. M., & Botero, J. C. (2011). Contextual predictive factors of child sexual abuse: the role of parent–child interaction. Child Abuse and Neglect, 35, 1022–1031.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, L. R., Morris, A. S., Heller, S. S., Scheeringa, M. S., Boris, N. W., & Smyke, A. T. (2009). Relations between emotion regulation, parenting and psychopathology in young maltreated children in out of home care. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 18, 421–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, L. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory of mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Development, 75, 655–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523–541.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jessica O’Reilly.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

O’Reilly, J., Peterson, C.C. Maltreatment and Advanced Theory of Mind Development in School-aged Children. J Fam Viol 30, 93–102 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10896-014-9647-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10896-014-9647-9

Keywords

Navigation