Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 2/2018

05-09-2017

Temperament, Repetitive Negative Thinking, and Depressive Symptoms in Early Adolescence: a Prospective Study

Auteurs: Patricia Bijttebier, Margot Bastin, Sabine Nelis, Sofie Weyn, Koen Luyckx, Michael W. Vasey, Filip Raes

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment | Uitgave 2/2018

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Repetitive negative thinking (RNT) is a transdiagnostic vulnerability factor, involved in the maintenance of a variety of emotional problems. Recently, the Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire – Child version (PTQ-C) was developed as a content-independent measure of RNT in children and adolescents. The current study investigated the reliability and predictive validity of the PTQ-C and examined temperament as a developmental predictor of RNT. For this end, 701 early adolescents completed measures of RNT, depressive symptoms, stressors, and temperament, both at baseline and after three months. First, the factor structure of the PTQ-C was investigated by comparing a one-factor model and a three-factor higher-order model using confirmatory factor analyses. Both models showed good fit to the data, but the more parsimonious one-factor model was retained. Internal consistency, as measured by cronbach’s alpha and (hierarchical) omega, was found to be excellent for the total scale. Second, associations with depressive symptom levels were examined. RNT predicted both concurrent and prospective symptom levels, even when taking into account baseline depressive symptoms. RNT was further shown to act as a moderator strengthening the link between stress and levels of depressive symptoms, both concurrently and prospectively. Finally, associations with temperament were explored by investigating the mediating role of RNT in the association between temperament dimensions and depressive symptoms. One indirect effect was found, with low effortful control predicting increases in depressive symptoms through heightened levels of RNT. The current study supports the PTQ-C as a useful and psychometrically sound measure of dysfunctional RNT that may facilitate research on emotional problems in child and adolescent samples.
Voetnoten
1
Unfortunately, we do not have any information on racial/ethnic background or SES. However, the population of children in the participating schools is predominantly Caucasian and can be considered as representative of the Belgian population in terms of SES.
 
2
We thank an anonymous reviewer for this suggestion.
 
3
To examine whether the one-factor structure of this model held across gender and age groups, sequential multi-group confirmatory factor analyses were conducted to test for configural invariance, metric invariance, and scalar invariance. Following the guidelines of Chen (2007), metric invariance was assessed if ΔCFI , ΔRMSEA, and ΔSRMR were below .010, .015, and .030, respectively. Scalar invariance was regarded if these indexes were below .010, .015, and .010, respectively. For age, we compared a group of children aged 9 to 11 ( [9.58-11.50]; n = 327) with a group of children aged 12 to 15 ([11.51-14.83]; n = 371)). Results revealed both metric (χ²diff (14)= 16.44, p = .29; ΔCFI < .001, ΔRMSEA =.003; ΔSRMR = .006) and scalar invariance (χ²diff (14)= 18.27, p = .20; ΔCFI = .001, ΔRMSEA =.002; ΔSRMR = .002). For gender as well, results revealed both metric (χ²diff (14)= 13.26, p = .51; ΔCFI < .001, ΔRMSEA =.002; ΔSRMR = .004) and scalar invariance (χ²diff (14)= 20.30, p = .12; ΔCFI = .002, ΔRMSEA =.002; ΔSRMR < .001). Thus, we could conclude this one-factor structure to be invariant across age and gender.
 
4
After running this model, a second model was run in which we also included the temperamental factors at T2 (resulting in the addition of stability paths for temperament, as well as the within-time associations with temperamental factors at T2). Model fit decreased, with χ2 (14) = 165.95, p < .001; CFI = .96; RMSEA =.12. The most apparent difference with the previous model was that the indirect effect from EC at T1 on CDI at T2 through PTQ-C disappeared (bias-corrected 95% CI [-.01, .00], p = .22; B = -.00, SE = .003). EC at T1 was still a significant predictor of the PTQ-C at T2, but the previously found association between PTQ-C at T1 and the CDI at T2 did not hold, B = .05, p = .12.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural models. Newbury Park: Sage Publications. Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural models. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.
go back to reference Engelen, U., De Peuter, S., Victoir, A., Van Diest, I., & Van den Bergh, O. (2006). Verdere validering van de positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) en vergelijking van twee Nederlandstalige versies. [further validation of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) and the comparison of two Dutch versions.] Gedrag & Gezondheid, 34, 89–102. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03087979. Engelen, U., De Peuter, S., Victoir, A., Van Diest, I., & Van den Bergh, O. (2006). Verdere validering van de positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) en vergelijking van twee Nederlandstalige versies. [further validation of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS) and the comparison of two Dutch versions.] Gedrag & Gezondheid, 34, 89–102. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​BF03087979.
go back to reference Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive behavioural processes across psychological disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive behavioural processes across psychological disorders. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Ingram, R.E., & Price, J.M. (2001). The role of vulnerability in understanding psychopathology. In R.E. Ingram & J.M. Price (Eds.), Vulnerability to psychopathology. Risk across the life span (pp. 3-19). New York: Guilford Press. Ingram, R.E., & Price, J.M. (2001). The role of vulnerability in understanding psychopathology. In R.E. Ingram & J.M. Price (Eds.), Vulnerability to psychopathology. Risk across the life span (pp. 3-19). New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Joormann, J. (2005). Inhibition, rumination, and mood regulation in depression. In R. W. Engle, G. Sedek, U. von Hecker, & D. N. McIntosh (Eds.), Cognitive limitations in aging and psychopathology: Attention, working memory, and executive functions (pp. 275–312). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511720413.012. Joormann, J. (2005). Inhibition, rumination, and mood regulation in depression. In R. W. Engle, G. Sedek, U. von Hecker, & D. N. McIntosh (Eds.), Cognitive limitations in aging and psychopathology: Attention, working memory, and executive functions (pp. 275–312). Cambridge University Press. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1017/​CBO9780511720413​.​012.
go back to reference Kline, R. B. (2006). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press. Kline, R. B. (2006). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.
go back to reference Kovacs, M. (2003). Children’s depression inventory: Technical manual. New York: Multi-health Systems. Kovacs, M. (2003). Children’s depression inventory: Technical manual. New York: Multi-health Systems.
go back to reference Lonigan, C. J., & Phillips, B. M. (2001). Temperamental influences on the development of anxiety disorders. In M. W. Vasey & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 60–91). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef Lonigan, C. J., & Phillips, B. M. (2001). Temperamental influences on the development of anxiety disorders. In M. W. Vasey & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 60–91). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: A unified treatment. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: A unified treatment. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
go back to reference Nagin, D. S. (2005). Group-based modelling of development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRef Nagin, D. S. (2005). Group-based modelling of development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2004). The response style theory. In C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination: Nature, theory and treatment (pp. 107–123). West Sussex: Wiley. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2004). The response style theory. In C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination: Nature, theory and treatment (pp. 107–123). West Sussex: Wiley.
go back to reference Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Girgus, J. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1992). Predictors and consequences of childhood depressive symptoms: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 405–422.CrossRefPubMed Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Girgus, J. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1992). Predictors and consequences of childhood depressive symptoms: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 405–422.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (pp. 105–176). New York: Wiley. Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (pp. 105–176). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Timbremont, B., Braet, C., & Roelofs, J. (2008). Handleiding children’s depression inventory (herziene versie) [manual for Children’s depression inventory (revised version)]. Amsterdam: Pearson. Timbremont, B., Braet, C., & Roelofs, J. (2008). Handleiding children’s depression inventory (herziene versie) [manual for Children’s depression inventory (revised version)]. Amsterdam: Pearson.
go back to reference Verstraeten, K., Vasey, M. W., Claes, L., & Bijttebier, P. (2010). The assessment of effortful control in childhood: Questionnaires and the test of everyday attention for children compared. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 59–65.CrossRef Verstraeten, K., Vasey, M. W., Claes, L., & Bijttebier, P. (2010). The assessment of effortful control in childhood: Questionnaires and the test of everyday attention for children compared. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 59–65.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Temperament, Repetitive Negative Thinking, and Depressive Symptoms in Early Adolescence: a Prospective Study
Auteurs
Patricia Bijttebier
Margot Bastin
Sabine Nelis
Sofie Weyn
Koen Luyckx
Michael W. Vasey
Filip Raes
Publicatiedatum
05-09-2017
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment / Uitgave 2/2018
Print ISSN: 0882-2689
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3505
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-017-9624-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2018

Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment 2/2018 Naar de uitgave