Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Child and Family Studies 12/2021

Open Access 08-10-2021 | Review

Cultural Differences in the Association of Harsh Parenting with Internalizing and Externalizing Symptoms: A Meta-Analysis

Auteur: Martin Pinquart

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Child and Family Studies | Uitgave 12/2021

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

The goal of the present meta-analysis was to compare associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing symptoms across regions of the globe and ethnic groups, and to search for moderator effects of indicators of cultural normativeness of harsh parenting. The systematic search in electronic databases and cross-referencing identified 971 studies. Random-effects meta-analyses were computed on bivariate and cross-lagged associations. Harsh parenting was associated with more internalizing and externalizing symptoms in all assessed regions of the globe and in all compared ethnic groups within western countries. Cross-lagged statistical effects of harsh parenting on change in child symptoms were found in North America, Western Europe, Latin America, East Asia, South-East Asia, and North Africa/the Arabian Peninsula, while there were no data from Eastern Europe. In line with the cultural normativeness hypothesis, a few moderating effects of the legal ban of physical punishment of children, acceptance and prevalence of physical punishment, and individualism-collectivism were identified. Externalizing symptoms predicted a stronger increase in harsh parenting if physical punishment was more accepted in the individual country. However, national levels of acceptance of physical punishment did not affect associations of harsh parenting with change in child outcomes. Although most associations of harsh parenting with child symptoms were statistically small (bivariate associations) or very small (cross-lagged associations), it is concluded that parents across the globe should be recommended to avoid harsh parenting. More longitudinal studies are needed for analyzing regional differences in parent and child effects.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10826-021-02113-z.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Data from UNICEF (2019) indicate that most children in Africa (82.5%) and Asia (76.8%) have experienced violent discipline (physical punishment or psychological aggression) in the last month. Rates were lower in Eastern Europe (63.5%) and Latin America (63.3%). A study from the US indicated that 80% of mothers of 6-year-olds have spanked their child (Gershoff et al., 2012). Severe forms of physical punishment tend to be less common than milder forms. For example, a study by Manzoni and Schwarzenegger (2019) asked 7th, 8th, and 9th graders from 26 countries whether one of their parents ever hit them with an object, punched them, kicked them forcefully, or beat them up. Rates were highest in Venezuela (15.8%), Indonesia (13.4%), and Cape Verde (12.9%) and lowest in Denmark (2.6%) and Kosovo (2.0%). A study with university students from 32 nations on perceived acceptance of physical punishment asked whether it is sometimes necessary to discipline the child “with a good harsh spanking.” While only 15% of the Swedish participants agreed, agreement rates of above 90% were found in South Korea and Singapore (Straus, 2010, p. 17). About 83% of Western European countries, 67% of Eastern European countries, 27% of Central and South American countries, 18% of Subsahara African countries, 11% of North African countries/the Arabian Peninsula, and 6% of Asian countries have legally banned all physical punishment of children by their parents (Global Initiative to End Corporal Punishment of Children, 2020). Nonetheless, physical punishment that does not cause harm or injury is still allowed in many countries, such as the United States (Global Initiative to End Corporal Punishment of Children, 2020; Miller-Perrin & Perrin, 2018). In sum, physical punishment of children tends to be least normative in Western Europe and most normative in Africa, the Arab Peninsula, and Asia (see also, Dwairy et al., 2006; Wang & Liu, 2018)
As physical punishment and negative verbal behavior of parents towards their children often cooccur, many studies use combined measures of overall harsh parenting (Hinnant et al., 2015). The term harsh parenting refers to coercive acts and negative emotional expressions of parents towards their children. It includes verbal aggression, such as yelling or name calling, and physical aggression, such as slapping, spanking, or beating up (Hinnant et al., 2015). Harsh parenting can be conceptualized along a continuum from acts causing minimal psychological and/or psychological pain to child maltreatment which causes serious injury or even death of the child (Gershoff, 2002). A number of meta-analyses have shown that harsh parenting in general (Pinquart, 2017a, b) and physical punishment in particular (Ferguson, 2013; Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016a; Larzerle et al., 2018) are concurrently associated with higher levels of externalizing and internalizing symptoms in children and adolescents, and that harsh parenting and physical punishment predict an increase in these symptoms over time. However, most of the included studies have been conducted in the US or in other western countries, and within Caucasian families. For example, in the meta-analysis by Gershoff and Grogan-Kaylor (2016a), 69.4% of the included effect sizes came from the US, and part of the remaining studies were from Western Europe. Thus, it has not been systematically tested whether associations of harsh parenting and child outcomes are the same across all regions of the globe.
It has been suggested that associations between harsh parenting and child outcomes vary depending on whether this behavior is culturally accepted (the cultural normativeness hypothesis; Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997; Lansford et al., 2005; Liu & Wang, 2018; Rohner et al., 1996). The basic assumption is that children interpret parental behavior in relation to the behavior of other parents in their community or culture, and react more positively towards normative parental behavior (that is accepted and prevalent in their culture) than towards other behaviors (Grusec & Goodnow, 1994; Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997). Applied to physical discipline, Lansford et al. (2005) suggested that physical punishment will have negative effects on child outcomes if it is perceived as nonnormative while it will have no negative effect if perceived as culturally normative. Similarly, Wang and Liu (2018, p. 175) proposed that in traditional Chinese societies, parental harsh discipline may be accepted because both parents and children perceive this behavior as indicators of parental involvement, concern, and love, as indicated by a Chinese proverb, which states “Beating and scolding is the emblem of love.” Under this condition, harsh parenting would not cause adjustment problems in the offspring. In western countries, such as the US or Western Europe, children may perceive harsh discipline as a sign of parental rejection, which may cause, again, anger and hostility in the child with accompanying opposition (Lansford et al., 2005; Lansford & Dodge, 2008, Rohner et al., 1996; Wang & Liu, 2018). Nonetheless, cultural acceptance of physical punishment or harsh parenting in general may not necessarily lead to weaker associations of these parental behaviors with child outcomes. More concretely, a decline in the acceptance of harsh parenting, such as physical punishment, is expected to lead to a lower prevalence of this behavior in general and of severe forms in particular (Janson et al., 2010). Severe forms of physical punishment have been found to show stronger associations with child outcomes than milder forms (Ferguson, 2013). If only few cases with milder forms of harsh punishment persist in a country in response to low cultural normativeness, variance restriction may lead to small associations with child outcomes.
Cultural normativeness could also play a role when addressing effects of children’s behavior problems on harsh parenting. Several theoretical models suggest that dysfunctional parenting and children’s problem behaviors reinforce each other (Bell, 1977; Reid et al., 2002). For example, if harsh parenting is more culturally accepted, parents may be more likely to use physically punishment after their child misbehaved because of expecting that physical punishment is appropriate for preventing future externalizing problems.
Available cross-national studies provided only partial support for the suggestion that indicators of normativeness moderate the size of the association of harsh parenting with child outcomes. A comparative study of associations between corporal punishment and behavior problems in six countries found the weakest association between maternal use of physical punishment and parental reports about children’s behavior problems in the countries in which corporal punishment was most prevalent and probably most culturally accepted (i.e., India, Kenya). However, no such difference was found when using child reports about anxiety and aggressive behavior as outcome variables (Lansford et al., 2005). Manzoni and Schwarzenegger (2019) found the strongest association between physical punishment and externalizing problems in the country with the lowest prevalence rate of this form of punishment (i.e., Kosovo). However, in the country with the highest prevalence rate (i.e., Venezuela), the association of physical punishment and externalizing problems was not below the average of all included 26 countries. A meta-analysis by Gershoff and Grogan-Kaylor (2016a) did not find differences between the results of studies from the US and those from outside the US, but the cultural normativeness hypothesis would indicate strongest differences between Western European countries and countries from Africa and Asia. Unfortunately, these studies did not directly test whether indicators of cultural normativeness explain national differences in the association of harsh parenting with child outcomes.
Three sources of information can be used on national differences in the acceptance of harsh parenting – cross-national surveys that assessed perceptions of normativeness of these parenting practices (Strauss, 2010), comparative studies on their actual use (actual normativeness, such as Manzoni & Schwarzenegger, 2019), and data on whether harsh parenting has been legally banned in individual countries (legal normativeness, Global Initiative to End Corporal Punishment of Children, 2020). With regard to cultural acceptance, some authors have suggested that the national level of collectivism-individualism might indicate whether mild forms of physical punishment are culturally appropriate for promoting obedience (Ifan, 2008; Rudy & Grusec, 2006). In collectivist cultures, individuals are integrated into strong in-groups (e.g., the nuclear family) with emphasis on cohesiveness and harmony, and in-group members protect each other in exchange for unquestioning loyalty (Hofstede et al., 2010). In this cultural context, power-assertive parental behaviors, such as mild forms of spanking, are acceptable for promoting obedience (Ifan, 2008; Rudy & Grusec, 2006). In contrast, more individualistic cultures promote independence of the individual (Hofstede et al., 2010), and harsh parenting hinders the development of child’s sense of autonomy (Grusec & Goodnow, 1994).
Cultural differences in the acceptance and use of harsh parenting also exist within countries, such as between ethnic groups. Ethnic groups are linked by a shared culture and social heritage (Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016b). African–American parents tend to report higher levels of spanking and to perceive spanking as more appropriate compared to European American parents (Geshoff et al., 2012; Lee & Watson, 2020; Silveira et al., 2021). Nonetheless, a meta-analysis by Gershoff and Grogan-Kaylor (2016b) did not find differences in the association of spanking with child outcomes between African–American and European American families. However, the test power was limited due to inclusion of only five studies. A meta-analysis has not yet addressed whether associations of harsh parenting and child outcomes differ between Caucasian, Hispanic, and Asian families. Comparisons of the use of harsh parenting by European American and Hispanic parents led to inconsistent results which varied by level of acculturation (Geshoff et al., 2012) and cohort membership (Taillieu et al., 2014), thus indicating that there is also variability within ethnic groups. Studies found that Chinese–American mothers have more positive attitudes towards spanking than Caucasian mothers (Mah & Johnston, 2012) and that Asian–Canadians use more physical punishment than Caucasian Canadians (Fréchette & Romano, 2015). However, a study by Gershoff et al. (2012) indicated similar rates of spanking and similar associations of spanking with externalizing problems in Asian–American and Caucasian American families. Differences in the ethnic composition of the samples may explain inconsistent results as Silveira et al. (2021) found higher rates of physical punishment in British–Asian (mainly Pakistani, Bangladeshi, Indian) parents compared to White parents while the reverse was found in the US where most Asian families were Chinese and Filipino.

Research Questions

Available meta-analyses have only addressed differences between associations of harsh parenting with child outcomes between the US and countries other than the US (Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016a), and between African–American and European American families (Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016b). Thus, there is a need for a more differentiated view on regions of the globe and for including other ethnic groups (e.g., Asian American and Hispanic families). The goal of the present meta-analysis was to compare associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing problems across 10 geographic regions and four ethnic groups, based on the cultural normativeness hypothesis. Because associations of harsh parenting with internalizing symptoms tend to be weaker than associations with externalizing symptoms (Pinquart, 2017a, b), they were analyzed separately. As very small numbers of studies are available for most countries, the present meta-analysis focused on regions rather than individual countries. Based on the categorization of regions according to common cultural roots (Huntington, 1996; Spencer & Thomas, 1973), ten regions were compared: North America, Central/Southern America, Western Europe (countries that have not been part of the former communist bloc), Eastern Europe/Russia, North Africa/the Arabian Peninsula, Sub-Sahara Africa, Australia/New Zealand, East Asia (e.g., China, Japan, Taiwan), South Asia (e.g., India, Bangladesh, Pakistan), and South-East Asia (e.g., Cambodia, Indonesia, Thailand). The frontiers of these regions are somewhat permeable as, for example, some formerly communist Eastern European countries have now become members of the European Union. In contrast to almost all African and Asian countries, all forms of physical punishment of children are prohibited in most Western European countries (Global Initiative to End Corporal Punishment of Children, 2020; UNICEF, 2019). Thus, the first research question asked whether associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing problems are weaker in African and Asian countries than in Western Europe.
The second research questions asked whether associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing symptoms would be stronger in countries that have legally banned all forms of physical punishment of children, in countries where physical punishment of children by their parents is less accepted and prevalent, and in the more individualistic/less collectivistic countries.
The third research question addressed ethnic differences within western countries and asked whether associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing symptoms would be stronger in Caucasian families than in families with an African ethnic background. Differences between Caucasian families and Hispanic families, as well as Asian families, were explored without stating a directional hypothesis because comparative data on the prevalence of harsh parenting in the latter groups were inconclusive (Gershoff et al., 2012; Silveira et al., 2021; Taillieu et al., 2014). While it would also be interesting to meta-analyze ethnic differences in non-western countries, there were not enough studies for doing this.
As bivariate associations between parenting and child behaviors could reflect parental effects as well as child effects (Bell, 1977; Reid et al., 2002), separate analyses of bivariate and cross-lagged associations between harsh parenting and internalizing/externalizing symptoms were computed.

Methods

Sample

Studies were found through the electronic databases PSYCINFO, ERIC, Google Scholar, and PSYNDEX [search terms: (harsh parenting OR spanking OR corporal punishment OR harsh discipline OR physical punishment) AND (parents OR mothers OR fathers) AND (internalizing problems OR externalizing problems OR delinqu* OR aggress* OR anxiety OR depress*)]. The references of the identified studies were checked for additional papers. The final search was completed on March 2, 2021. Criteria for inclusion of studies in the present meta-analysis were:
a.
Correlations of harsh parenting (e.g., yelling, shouting, spanking, slapping) with internalizing problems and/or externalizing problems were reported or could be computed.
 
b.
The mean age of the sample was <20 years, based on the age range of childhood and adolescence proposed by UNICEF.
 
c.
The studies were completed before March, 2021.
 
Studies were excluded if they
a.
provided only multivariate analyses, as these effect sizes cannot be combined with bivariate coefficients, and
 
b.
used only a sum-score of negative parenting which also included behaviors others than harsh parenting.
 
The included studies were not limited to those written in English. Unpublished studies from the databases (e.g., dissertations) were included in order to minimize effects of publication bias. The search identified 1804 records. After screening and assessing for eligibility, 971 studies were included in the meta-analysis, with 41 of them not being published in English (Fig. 1). Selected characteristics of the included studies are reported in the supplementary appendices A1 and A2.
The present meta-analysis extends previous work (Pinquart, 2017a, b) by focusing on cultural similarities and differences. The meta-analysis has not been preregistered. As moderator effects of some study characteristics (e.g., age, source of information, study quality) have already been reported in previous papers, they were not included in the present study. The meta-analysis followed the PRISMA 2009 checklist. A review protocol is available from the author.
The following variables from the studies were entered: Number of children, mean age, percentage of girls, percentage of children with African, Asian, European/Caucasian, and Hispanic ethnic backgrounds, country and region of data collection, method for assessing harsh parenting, method for assessing internalizing/externalizing symptoms, support for validity of the parenting and symptom measures (1 = yes, 0 = no), study design (1 = cross-sectional, 2 = longitudinal) and the size of bivariate and cross-lagged association of harsh parenting with internalizing/externalizing problems. Cross-lagged effect sizes (that control for the pretest score of the independent variable) often had to be computed from the reported matrix of bivariate correlations. If the same effect size was reported in more than one paper, the effect size was coded only once. The author coded all studies. A graduate student also coded about 20% of the studies. A mean inter-rater reliability of 92% (range 88–100%) was established; differences were resolved by discussion.

Measures

The studies assessed harsh parenting with the Parent-to-Child Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus et al., 1998; 170 studies), the Parenting Scale (Arnold et al., 1993; 83 studies), the Alabama Parenting Questionnaire (Frick, 1991; 81 studies), observations (60 studies), and other related instruments (578 studies). Internalizing symptoms were assessed with the Child Behavior Problem Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991; 148 studies), the Youth Self-Report (YSR; Achenbach, 1991; 46 studies), the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, 1997; 42 studies), as well as other related instruments (211 studies). Externalizing symptoms were measured with the CBCL (267 studies), the YSR (70 studies), the SDQ (51 studies), the Teacher Report Form (Achenbach, 1991; 40 studies), and other related instruments (440 studies).
For the individual countries in which the studies took place, national levels of four variables were extracted from the following sources: The Global Initiative to End All Corporal Punishment of Children (2020) provided national data on whether corporal punishment in the family was prohibited in the individual countries (2 = yes, 1 = no). National acceptance rates of physical punishment were taken from Straus (2010) who had asked whether it is sometimes necessary to discipline the child “with a good harsh spanking”. The third International Self-Report Delinquency Study provided adolescent reports from 30 countries on whether they have been beaten, slapped, or pushed by their mothers or fathers in the past 12 months (Enzmann, 2018). Scores from Enzmann (2018) and Straus (2010) for the countries of the present meta-analysis are provided in Appendix A3. Finally, the country levels of individualism-collectivism were extracted from Hofstede Insights (2019); higher scores represent higher levels of individualism. Prevalence rates of harsh parenting from the Multiple Indicator Cluster Survey (UNICEF, 2019) could not be used because this survey had not included the countries from which most studies on associations of harsh parenting and internalizing/ externalizing problems were available, such as the U.S., China, or Canada.

Statistical Analysis

The meta-analytic procedures were similar to those of a previous meta-analysis on cultural differences in associations of parenting styles with child outcomes (Pinquart & Kauser, 2018). Calculations were performed in five steps. Random-effects models with the method of moments were computed (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001).
1.
Correlations were transformed into Fisher’s z-scores. Then, outliers that were more than two SDs from the mean of the effect sizes were recoded to the value at two SDs, based on Lipsey and Wilson (2001).
 
2.
Weighted mean z-scores and 95%-confidence intervals [CIs] were computed. The significance of the mean effect size was tested by dividing the weighted mean effect size by the standard error of the mean. Mean z-scores were later converted to the original metric of product–moment correlations.
 
3.
Homogeneity of effect sizes was assessed by use of the Q statistic and I2.
 
4.
In order to test differences between regions, an analog of the analysis of variance was used. A significant Q score indicates heterogeneity between regions. Differences between two regions are significant if their CIs do not overlap (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001). Weighted linear regressions (meta-regressions) were used for analyzing moderating effects of normativeness of harsh parenting.
 
5.
Egger’s test and trim-and-fill analysis were used for detecting a possible publication bias (Duval & Tweedie, 2000).
 

Results

The included studies have been conducted in 76 countries, with most studies coming from the United States (N = 535), China (N = 67), Canada (N = 40), and the Netherlands (N = 38) (see Appendix A 3 for a full list of the countries). The studies provided data on 984,520 parent–child dyads. The children and adolescents had a mean age of 8.48 years (SD = 4.50). About 48.7% of the children were female; 133 of the included studies were unpublished. About 86.7% of the studies used community-based convenience samples. While 83.6% of the studies provided support for the validity of assessment of child symptoms, the percentage was lower for parenting measures (60.6%), where many studies used single-item indicators of spanking frequency. The mean follow-up interval of longitudinal studies was 2.91 years (SD = 2.45).

Regional Differences

Because the size of bivariate associations was significantly larger in cross-sectional studies than in longitudinal studies (internalizing: r = 0.17 vs. r = 0.11, Q(1) = 42.24, p < 0.001; externalizing: r = 0.24 vs. r = 0.18, Q(1) = 76.67, p < 0.001), separate analyses were computed for both kinds of studies.
With regard to the first research question, there were significant concurrent associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing symptoms in all regions (Table 1). Nonetheless, there was significant regional variability: Associations with internalizing symptoms were stronger in East Asia than in Subsahara Africa and Western Europe. The size of this association with externalizing symptoms was stronger in South Asia than in other regions, except for Latin America. Associations were also stronger in Latin America and Subsahara Africa than in South-East Asia.
Table 1
Regional differences in the association of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing problems
   
Internalizing problems
Externalizing problems
 
k
r
95%-CI
Z
Q
I2
k
r
95%-CI
Z
Q
I2
Bivariate concurrent associations
All studies
740
0.17
0.16
0.18
38.48c
687.52
0
1432
0.24
0.23
0.24
65.24c
1341.02
0
Regions
22.85b
      
56.26c
 
North America
444
0.17
0.16
0.19
27.99c
384.86
0
784
0.23
0.22
0.24
43.93c
695.67
0
Western Europe
103
0.16
0.14
0.18
14.43c
93.05
0
256
0.24
0.23
0.26
29.15c
241.05
0
Eastern Europe
17
0.17
0.12
0.21
6.55c
12.35
0
44
0.25
0.23
0.30
14.56c
29.52
0
Latin America
20
0.14
0.09
0.18
5.90c
16.78
0
34
0.28
0.24
0.32
13.53c
29.52
0
East Asia
78
0.21
0.19
0.24
16.41c
80.16
3.9
160
0.22
0.20
0.24
20.83c
140.94
0
South Asia
8
0.24
0.16
0.32
5.74c
18.06a
61.2
27
0.36
0.31
0.41
13.79c
30.82
15.6
South-East Asia
10
0.21
0.14
0.28
5.69c
11.02
18.3
31
0.16
0.12
0.20
6.87c
29.85
0
NA/AP
9
0.19
0.12
0.26
5.01c
8.25
0
35
0.24
0.21
0.28
11.61c
34.03
0
Sub-Sahara
18
0.13
0.08
0.17
5.18c
18.69
0.9
23
0.25
0.21
0.31
9.27c
28.14
21.8
Australia/ New Zeal.
29
0.15
0.10
0.19
6.37c
21.47
0
29
0.27
0.23
0.32
11.26c
26.23
0
Bivariate longitudinal associations of harsh parenting (t1) with behavior problems (t2)
All studies
195
0.11
0.09
0.13
12.15c
158.74
0
578
0.18
0.17
0.19
30.05c
482.66
0
Regions
     
3.34
      
19.94b
 
North America
144
0.10
0.08
0.11
10.96c
115.42
0
417
0.18
0.17
0.19
29.03c
349.49
0
Western Europe
22
0.13
0.09
0.16
6.69c
11.48
0
92
0.19
0.16
0.22
13.95c
63.86
0
Eastern Europe
              
Latin America
       
1
0.08
−0.14
0.29
0.71
0.00
0
East Asia
20
0.12
0.08
0.16
6.02c
23.42
18.9
39
0.14
0.10
0.18
6.87c
27.36
0
South Asia
              
South-East Asia
1
0.15
0.03
0.26
2.44a
0.00
0
8
0.08
0.01
0.15
2.23a
8.51
17.7
NA/AP
       
7
0.27
0.19
0.34
6.56c
3.46
0
Sub-Sahara
       
3
0.06
−0.24
0.35
0.37
0.49
0
Australia/New Zeal
8
0.09
0.03
0.14
3.11b
2.09
0
11
0.15
0.09
0.20
4.84c
9.56
0
Harsh parenting – change of behavioral problems
All studies
126
0.05
0.04
0.07
8.35c
105.08
0
428
0.08
0.07
0.09
16.31c
329.68
0
Regions
     
12.75a
      
31.20c
 
North America
92
0.05
0.04
0.07
6.81c
71.31
0
280
0.08
0.07
0.09
13.88c
222.09
0
Western Europe
14
0.07
0.03
0.10
4.06c
13.50
3.7
84
0.09
0.06
0.11
7.26c
50.73
0
Eastern Europe
              
Latin America
1
0.08
0.00
0.16
1.98a
0.00
0
1
0.28
0.18
0.39
5.13c
0.00
0
East Asia
13
0.04
0.01
0.08
2.41a
7.19
0
40
0.06
0.03
0.09
3.75c
16.39
0
South Asia
              
South-East Asia
1
0.09
0.02
0.16
2.40a
0.00
0
10
0.03
−0.01
0.07
1.29
4.12
0
NA/AP
       
6
0.10
0.03
0.17
2.80b
3.21
0
Sub-Sahara
1
−0.10
−0.19
−0.00
−2.02a
0.00
0
4
−0.08
−0.20
0.04
−1.35
1.22
0
Australia/New Zeal
4
0.01
−0.08
0.11
0.29
0.33
0
3
−0.00
−0.11
0.10
−0.02
0.71
0
Behavioral problems – change of harsh parenting
All studies
68
0.05
0.03
0.08
4.64c
57.09
0
244
0.09
0.07
0.10
14.51c
184.36
 
Regions
     
3.47
      
18.04a
 
North America
55
0.05
0.03
0.08
4.24c
51.44
0
148
0.08
0.06
0.09
10.65c
127.58
0
Western Europe
5
0.01
−0.06
0.08
0.22
1.49
0
60
0.13
0.10
0.15
8.97c
30.95
0
Latin America
1
0.03
−0.11
0.16
0.37
 
0
1
0.13
0.03
0.23
2.46a
0.00
0
East Asia
6
0.11
0.03
0.19
2.60b
0.68
0
20
0.07
0.02
0.12
2.72b
3.94
0
South Asia
              
South-East Asia
       
5
0.03
−0.03
0.08
0.90
0.69
0
NA/AP
       
6
0.14
0.07
0.20
4.07c
2.19
0
Sub-Sahara
       
3
0.24
−0.04
0.50
1.67
0.98
0
Australia/New Zeal
1
0.09
−0.38
0.52
0.35
0.00
0
1
0.08
−0.06
0.22
1.08
0.00
0
k = number of effect sizes included; r = weighted mean correlation coefficient; Z = test for significance of r. 95 % CI = lower and upper limits of 95% confidence interval; Q = test for homogeneity of effect sizes; I2 = proportion of the observed variance that reflects variance in true effect sizes; NA/AP = North Africa/Arabian Peninsula
ap < 0.05
bp < 0.01
cp < 0.001
The size of longitudinal associations of initial harsh parenting with internalizing symptoms at follow-up did not differ between regions. In contrast, there was significant regional variation in the size of associations of initial harsh parenting with externalizing symptoms at follow-up, with stronger associations in North Africa/the Arabian Peninsula than in North America, East Asia, and South-East Asia. In addition, associations were stronger in North America and Western Europe than in South-East Asia.
Cross-lagged analyses showed that initial harsh parenting predicted an increase in internalizing problems in the total sample as well as in North America, Western Europe, Latin America, East Asia, and South-East Asia. Interestingly, there was a significant negative association in Sub-Sahara Africa, but this association was based on only one study and may, therefore, not be reliable. The cross-lagged association was significantly more negative in Subsahara Africa than in the other regions, except Australia/New Zealand. Initial harsh parenting predicted an increase in externalizing symptoms in North America, Western Europe, Latin America, East Asia, and North Africa/the Arabian Peninsula. Regional variation was significant, and the association was stronger in North America, Western Europe, and Latin America than in Sub-Sahara Africa.
Cross-lagged associations of initial internalizing symptoms with change in harsh parenting did not vary by region. Internalizing symptoms predicted an increase in parental harshness in North America and East Asia. Initial externalizing symptoms predicted an increase in harsh parenting in North America, Western Europe, Latin America, East Asia, and North Africa/the Arabian Peninsula. There was regional variation in the size of the associations, with associations being stronger in Western Europe than in North America and South-East Asia. With the exception of concurrent associations of harsh parenting with internalizing symptoms in South Asia, there was no significant variation of the effect sizes within regions.
Egger’s tests indicated some funnel plot asymmetry in five analyses (Appendix A4). The trim-and-fill algorithm led to very small declines of the longitudinal association of harsh parenting with internalizing symptoms (from r = 0.11 to r = 0.09) and externalizing symptoms at follow-up (from r = 0.18 to r = 0.17) and of harsh parenting with change in externalizing symptoms (from r = 0.08 to 0.07). In contrast, the association of harsh parenting with change in internalizing symptoms and the association of externalizing symptoms with change in harsh parenting slightly increased (from r = 0.05 to r = 0.07 and from r = 0.09 to 0.10; Appendix A4).

Moderator Effects of Indicators of Normativeness of Harsh Parenting

The second research question asked whether country-level indicators of normativeness of harsh parenting (and of physical punishment in particular) moderated the size of associations between harsh parenting and child symptoms—namely the legal ban of physical punishment of children in the home, acceptance of physical punishment, prevalence of physical punishment, and the levels of individualism-collectivism.
Indicators of normativeness were correlated at the national level. The legal ban of physical punishment in the family was associated with lower national acceptance and prevalence rates of physical punishment, as well as with higher individualism-scores (Table 2). In addition, acceptance rates were lower in more individualistic countries.
Table 2
Correlations of National Levels of Acceptance and Use of Physical Punishment, Individualism-Collectivism, and Legal Ban of all Kind of Physical Punishment in the Home
Variables
Acceptance of physical punishment (Straus, 2010)
Prevalence of physical punishment (Enzmann, 2018)
Individualism-collectivism
Legal ban (1 = yes, 0 = no)
−0.40* (N = 26)
−0.52* (N = 27)
0.31* (N = 68)
Acceptance of physical punishment (Straus, 2010)
 
0.24 (N = 10)
−0.41* (N = 27)
Prevalence of physical punishment (Enzmann, 2018)
  
−0.26 (N = 26)
Higher scores on the individualism-collectivism scale indicate higher individualism. N indicates the number of countries. Data on legal ban were available for 75 of the 76 included countries, on acceptance and prevalence for 27 countries, and on individualism-collectivism for 69 countries
*p < 0.05
As shown in Table 3, the bivariate associations of harsh parenting with externalizing symptoms were significantly stronger in countries that have banned physical punishment of children in the home. Two moderating effects of the national level of acceptance of physical punishment of children were identified (Table 3). Bivariate concurrent correlations of harsh parenting with externalizing symptoms and associations of externalizing symptoms with an increase in harsh parenting were weaker in countries where physical punishment was more accepted.
Table 3
Moderating effects of indicators of country-levels of normativeness of physical punishment and individualism/collectivism on the association of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing problems
 
Internalizing problems
Externalizing problems
Moderator
k
B
β
Z
R2
k
B
β
Z
R2
Legal ban of physical punishment in the family (Global Initiative to End All Corporal Punishment of Children, 2020)
Bivariate concurrent assoc.
735
0.012
0.04
0.99
0.00
1422
0.033
0.09
3.27b
0.01
Bivariate longitudinal assoc.
195
0.031
0.10
1.29
0.01
578
−0.018
−0.04
−0.88
0.00
Harsh – change behav. probl.
126
0.030
0.12
1.17
0.01
428
0.009
0.03
0.45
0.00
Behav. probl. – change harsh
68
−0.008
−0.02
−0.14
0.00
244
−0.044
−0.10
−1.29
0.01
Acceptance of physical punishment (Straus, 2010)
Bivariate concurrent assoc.
669
0.001
0.07
1.62
0.00
1274
−0.001
−0.07
−2.31a
0.01
Bivariate longitudinal assoc.
194
0.000
−0.03
−0.40
0.00
562
−0.000
−0.01
−0.22
0.00
Harsh – change behav. probl.
126
−0.000
−0.04
−0.42
0.00
412
−0.000
−0.03
−0.47
0.00
Behav. probl. – change harsh
68
0.001
0.11
0.79
0.01
235
−0.002
−0.19
−2.40a
0.03
National prevalence of physical punishment (Enzmann, 2018)
    
Bivariate concurrent assoc.
487
0.001
0.02
0.33
0.00
1017
−0.003
−0.09
−2.67b
0.01
Bivariate longitudinal assoc.
153
−0.003
−0.10
−1.04
0.01
476
−0.001
−0.04
−0.73
0.00
Harsh – change behav. probl.
94
−0.001
−0.08
−0.64
0.01
347
−0.002
−0.09
−1.47
0.01
Behav. probl. – change harsh
50
0.004
0.15
0.99
0.02
204
−0.004
−0.17
−2.16a
0.03
Individualism (Hofstede Insights, 2019)
     
Bivariate concurrent assoc.
729
−0.000
−0.07
−1.80
0.00
1415
−0.000
−0.03
−1.04
0.00
Bivariate longitudinal assoc.
194
−0.000
−0.10
−1.19
0.00
578
0.001
0.10
2.22a
0.01
Harsh – change behav. probl.
125
−0.000
−0.06
−0.62
0.00
427
0.000
0.09
1.46
0.01
Behav. probl. – change harsh
68
−0.001
−0.13
−0.95
0.02
244
0.000
0.03
0.44
0.00
k = number of studies; B/β non-/standardized regression coefficient, t = test for significance, R2 = explained variance
ap < 0.05
bp < 0.01
Two moderating effects of the national prevalence of physical punishment were statistically significant: Bivariate concurrent associations of harsh parenting with externalizing symptoms were weaker in countries with a higher prevalence of physical punishment. In addition, initial externalizing symptoms predicted a weaker increase in harsh parenting if physical punishment was more prevalent in the country.
The present meta-analysis also found a moderating effect of the national level of individualism-collectivism: Longitudinal associations of harsh parenting with externalizing symptoms at follow-up were stronger in more individualistic countries.

Ethnic Differences in the Association of Harsh Parenting with Child Outcomes

The third research question asked whether the associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing problems differ in western countries between families with African, Asian, Caucasian, and Hispanic ethnic backgrounds. As shown in Table 4, the Q-tests indicated that bivariate and cross-lagged associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing symptoms did not differ between these ethnic groups. Harsh parenting was concurrently associated with higher levels of both kinds of symptoms in all compared ethnic groups—with small mean effect sizes (Cohen, 1992). Higher initial harsh parenting predicted higher internalizing and externalizing symptoms at follow-up in all groups, with the exception of families with Asian ethnicities. In addition, harsh parenting predicted very small increases of internalizing symptoms in Caucasian families and in those with an African ethnic background, as well as very small increases of externalizing symptoms in Caucasian, African, and Hispanic families. Furthermore, higher levels of externalizing problems predicted an increase in harsh parenting in families with Caucasian and African ethnic backgrounds. All but one effect size were homogeneous.
Table 4
Ethnic differences in the association of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing problems
 
Internalizing problems
Externalizing problems
 
k
r
95%-CI
Z
Q
I2
k
r
95%-CI
Z
Q
I2
Bivariate concurrent associations
0.72
      
4.01
 
Caucasian
20
0.16
0.10
0.22
5.34c
13.73
0
37
0.19
0.15
0.23
8.62c
35.98
0
African
14
0.16
0.10
0.21
5.44c
6.73
0
29
0.16
0.12
0.21
6.89c
31.73
11.8
Hispanic
37
0.19
0.14
0.23
7.22c
30.40
0
43
0.23
0.18
0.27
9.67c
29.58
0
Asian
23
0.16
0.10
0.22
5.39c
26.21
16.1
24
0.18
0.12
0.25
5.83c
26.41
12.9
Bivariate longitudinal associations
0.16
      
0.91
 
Caucasian
5
0.11
0.04
0.17
3.20b
0.78
0
14
0.15
0.10
0.20
5.43c
13.82
0
African
8
0.10
0.06
0.15
4.30c
5.22
0
21
0.15
0.10
0.20
6.14c
22.63
0
Hispanic
32
0.09
0.04
0.15
3.52c
7.05
0
41
0.18
0.13
0.23
7.13c
16.68
0
Asian
2
0.09
−0.03
0.21
1.45
0.03
0
1
0.18
−0.04
0.40
1.58
0.00
0
Harsh parenting – change behavior problems
0.57
      
1.34
 
Caucasian
6
0.02
0.01
0.04
2.65b
2.43
0
10
0.05
0.02
0.07
3.43c
12.24
26.5
African
5
0.03
0.00
0.06
2.07a
4.50
11.1
13
0.06
0.03
0.09
3.84c
6.70
0
Hispanic
17
0.02
−0.01
0.05
1.16
2.36
0
26
0.05
0.02
0.08
2.95b
14.74
0
Asian
4
0.03
−0.02
0.08
1.24
1.56
0
3
0.02
−0.04
0.08
0.73
6.03a
66.8
Behav. problems - change harsh parenting
0.70
      
4.80
 
Caucasian
2
0.01
−0.01
0.03
1.00
0.00
0
6
0.05
0.02
0.08
3.30c
9.58
47.8
African
2
0.02
−0.02
0.05
1.11
0.02
0
6
0.08
0.04
0.11
4.38c
10.34
51.6
Hispanic
7
0.02
−0.01
0.05
1.19
1.41
0
12
0.04
−0.00
0.07
1.80
8.41
0
Asian
3
−0.00
−0.05
0.05
−0.02
3.08
35.1
3
0.01
−0.05
0.07
0.38
0.03
0
Only studies from North America, Western Europe, and Australia were included in the analysis. k = number of effect sizes included; r = weighted mean correlation coefficient; Z = test for significance of r. 95% CI = lower and upper limits of 95% confidence interval; Q = test for homogeneity of effect sizes, I2 = proportion of the observed variance that reflects variance in true effect sizes
ap < 0.05
bp < 0.01
cp < 0.001

Discussion

The present meta-analysis compared the size of associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing symptoms across geographic regions and ethnic groups. There were more similarities than differences across the compared groups. Bivariate associations of harsh parenting with externalizing symptoms were stronger in South Asia than in most other regions. In addition, there were some moderating effects of the legal ban of physical punishment, acceptance of physical punishment, national prevalence of physical punishment, and level of individualism-collectivism.
Before interpreting the main results, some thoughts are needed about the validity of the data from the included studies. While the predominant use of convenience samples rather than nationally representative samples could lead to biased estimates of the prevalence of harsh parenting, this predominant sampling strategy is less relevant for the size of correlations of parenting with child outcomes as long as both variables show sufficient variation within each sample. Almost 40% of the studies had not provided support for the validity of their parenting measure, mainly when assessing the frequency of spanking with one or two items. Although these items have face validity, measurement errors tend to be larger. In fact, I had reported in an earlier study that correlations of harsh parenting with child outcomes were slightly smaller when using measures with unproven validity and reliability (Pinnquart, 2017b). When considering the quality of the data, it also has to be considered whether national bans of physical punishment could lead to an underreport of this behavior. However, data were usually collected anonymously, and available studies found substantial numbers of physical punishment despite national ban in the respective country (Enzmann, 2018).
Although there are national, regional, and ethnic differences in the acceptance and use of harsh parenting and of physical punishment in particular (Geshoff et al., 2012; Lee & Watson, 2020; Manzoni & Schwarzenegger, 2019; Strauss, 2010; UNICEF, 2019), associations of harsh parenting with child symptoms were found to be more similar across regions and ethnic groups. The rather universal associations with negative child outcomes may indicate that harsh parenting disregards important needs of the child, such as protection, warmth, and support of autonomy, which can lead to negative child outcomes (Global Initiative to End All Corporal Punishment of Children; Wang & Kenny, 2014).
The mean bivariate and cross-lagged associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing symptoms were similar to the results of previous meta-analyses, although most of them had a narrower focus on physical punishment (Ferguson, 2013; Gershoff & Grogan-Kaylor, 2016a; Larzerle et al., 2018; Pinquart, 2017a, b). In contrast to Gershoff and Grogan-Kaylor (2016a), the present study found smaller effect sizes in longitudinal studies than in cross-sectional studies. Smaller longitudinal effect sizes are explained by changes in the predictor variable over time leading to a decrease of predictive power of the initial level of the independent variable, and by the fact that concurrent correlations were based on bidirectional effects of the two assessed variables (Adachi & Willoughby, 2015).
As the included studies varied in whether they assessed harsh parenting as physical punishment and/or verbally aggressive behavior towards the child (e.g., Arnold et al., 1993; Frick, 1991; Straus et al., 1998), it is relevant to check whether the results of these studies can be combined. The fact that almost all mean effect sizes were homogeneous indicates that the results are similar across different indicators of harsh parenting. This result is not surprising as parental physical aggression towards children is often accompanied by verbal aggression. For example, Straus et al. (1998) reported that “ordinary corporal punishment” and “ordinary psychological aggression” correlated at r = 0.56.
Although all forms of physical punishment have mostly been banned in Western Europe (Global Initiative to End Corporal Punishment of Children, 2020), the present meta-analysis did not find that the associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing symptoms were strongest in Western Europe. However, parental physical punishment has not yet been completely, legally banned in four of the fifteen Western European countries that provided data for the present meta-analysis (Global Initiative to End Corporal Punishment of Children, 2020), thus indicating acceptance or tolerance in some Western European countries. In addition, a minority of parents from western countries still uses physical punishment, despite the legal ban in their country (Enzmann, 2018), thus indicating that a legal ban does not always lead to zero acceptance. In addition, acceptance of physical punishment decreased in countries outside Western Europe (Lansford et al., 2017), thus increasing the similarity between Western European and other countries. Finally, differences between Western Europe and other regions may be smaller with regard to nonphysical forms of harsh parenting, which were also included in the present meta-analysis.
The present meta-analysis found most of the observed regional variation in studies on externalizing problems. This result indicates that cultural variations in the normativeness of harsh parenting refer to the use of this behavior for changing delinquent and aggressive behavior rather than for dealing with negative feelings (Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997). The observed strongest concurrent associations of harsh parenting with externalizing problems in South Asian countries cannot be explained by cultural normativeness. Physical punishment of children tends to be widely accepted in these countries (Holden & Ashif, 2016; Straus, 2010; UNICEF, 2019), which should—according to the cultural normativeness hypothesis—reduce aversive effects of physical punishment. Similarly, the observed strongest longitudinal bivariate association of harsh parenting with externalizing symptoms in North Africa/the Arabian Peninsula cannot be explained by cultural normativeness because physical punishment tends to be culturally accepted in these regions (Dwairy et al., 2006; Ifan, 2008). Severe punishment is very common in these areas (and in rural parts in particular; Alyahri & Goodman, 2008; Holden & Ashif, 2016), and more severe physical punishment has been found to lead to more severe aversive consequences (Ferguson, 2013). This might have led to the above-average associations with child outcomes in these regions.
The small negative cross-lagged associations of harsh parenting with change in internalizing symptoms in Sub-Sahara Africa is more in line with the suggestion that harsh parenting may have less negative or even some positive effects if it is culturally accepted, as many parents from that region physically punish their children (UNICEF, 2019). The observed smaller association of externalizing problems with change in harsh parenting in South-East Asia might be related to the higher involvement of nonparental caregivers who intervene after externalizing behavior instead of the parents (Huang et al., 2015).
It is necessary to analyze moderating effects of indicators of the normativeness of harsh parenting at the country-level because comparative studies found some variation in the use and acceptance of physical punishment within the compared regions (Enzmann, 2018; Manzoni & Schwarzenegger, 2019; Straus, 2010; UNICEF, 2019). Bivariate analyses found some support for the suggestion that harsh parenting would show weaker associations with child outcomes if physical punishment is more accepted and prevalent in the individual country. This could mean that higher cultural acceptance increases the willingness to use harsh parenting in response to child symptoms (e.g., when the child misbehaves) and/or that harsh parenting has less or even no negative consequences on child outcomes if being culturally accepted (Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997; Lansford et al., 2005). Cross-lagged analyses supported only the former assumption. Thus, the present data indicate that cultural acceptance of physical punishment affects the reactive use of harsh parenting in response to externalizing behavior. However, as most tests for moderator effects of acceptance and prevalence of physical punishment were nonsignificant, the data indicate more similarities than differences. More moderating effects may emerge if representative national data on acceptance and prevalence of different forms of harsh parenting become available for all countries, and if data on acceptance/use and correlates of forms of harsh parenting are collected from the same samples. Such moderating effects have been found in some studies (Gershoff et al., 2010; Liu & Wang, 2018) but not in others (Alampay et al., 2017).
Only one out of eight analyses found empirical support for the suggestion that children from more collectivistic countries would suffer less from harsh parenting (Irfan, 2008; Rudy & Grusec, 2006). As social harmony is highly valued in collectivist societies (Hofstede et al., 2010), harsh parenting does not satisfy the child’s need for harmony, acceptance, and protection, which probably contributes to negative effects of harsh parenting in collectivistic countries.
The present meta-analysis extends the work of Gershoff and Grogan-Kaylor (2016b) by showing that, in Western countries, associations of harsh parenting with internalizing and externalizing problems are not only similar in families with Caucasian and African ethnic backgrounds, but also in those with Hispanic and Asian backgrounds. The observed similarity could be based on the fact that associations of harsh parenting with child outcomes are already rather similar across the regions of origin and on processes of acculturation that further increase similarities between ethnic groups within the individual country (Ho, 2014).

Limitations and Conclusions

Some limitations of the present meta-analysis must be mentioned: First, the studies addressed different forms of harsh parenting. However, as almost all effect sizes were homogeneous, the results were similar across the combined studies. Second, although harsh parenting was related to higher internalizing and externalizing symptoms in all regions, it could not be tested whether this is also true for each individual country. Unfortunately, the literature search only found relevant studies from about 39% of the countries in the world. Third, linear associations of harsh parenting with child outcomes were analyzed. A few studies indicate that harsh parenting may have a nonlinear relationship with externalizing problems (e.g., Stoolmiller et al., 1997). Fourth, data on legal ban, acceptance, and prevalence refer only to physical punishment, although many included studies had assessed harsh parenting more broadly. In addition, national data on acceptance/use of physical punishment and on associations of harsh parenting with internalizing/externalizing problems usually did not come from the same studies. Both factors reduce the size of moderating effects. Fifth, national data on the prevalence and acceptance of harsh physical punishment were only available for less than half of the countries that had provided studies for the present meta-analysis. The national samples that provided data on acceptance and use of harsh parenting were probably not representative for all parents of the individual countries. In addition, data on acceptance, use, and the legal ban of physical punishment and on collectivism-individualism were only available from one point of measurement, while the included studies were completed at different times. Sixth, only one source of differences within countries was addressed—ethnicity. Finally, the analysis of correlational data does not allow for drawing causal conclusions, as unmeasured third variables may explain, at least in part, the observed associations.
Despite these limitations, several conclusions can be drawn. First, although there is some regional variability in the association of harsh parenting with child symptoms, there are more similarities than differences. When forms of harsh parenting are more common in a particular region or ethnic group (Geshoff et al., 2012; Lee & Watson, 2020; Manzoni & Schwarzenegger, 2019; Strauss, 2010; UNICEF, 2019), this does not necessarily mean that harsh parenting will have less aversive effects. The present meta-analysis found no support for the assumption that higher cultural acceptance of physical punishment weakens negative effects of harsh parenting on change in child outcomes. Second, the data indicate that cultural acceptance of physical punishment promotes the reactive use of harsh parenting in response to externalizing symptoms of the child. Third, as harsh parenting was concurrently associated with more internalizing and externalizing symptoms in all regions of the globe and in all compared ethnic groups, the present results indicate that parents should not use harsh parenting. Multimodal interventions aimed at reducing harsh parenting may include public education campaigns that inform the public about negative effects of harsh parenting on the child, and parenting programs that teach non-harsh forms of parenting. The best available evidence for intervention effects on the reduction of parental physical punishment comes from indicated interventions with parents who have used more severe forms of harsh physical punishment in the past (e.g., Parent–Child Interaction Therapy), and from selective prevention for parents at risk, such as the Healthy Families home visitation program (Gershoff et al., 2017).
Fourth, with regard to future research needs, more longitudinal research is needed from most regions for separating parent and child effects. In particular, the observed association of harsh parenting with a decrease of internalizing symptoms in Sub-Sahara Africa calls for more studies that test whether this result can be replicated. Because some of the observed regional differences in the association of harsh parenting with child outcomes could not be explained by cultural normativeness, more cross-national research is needed that identifies other sources of variability across countries and regions.

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10826-021-02113-z.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that he has no potential conflicts of interest. No funding was received to assist with the preparation of this manuscript. The author did not collect data from human participants and/or animals. No formal ethical consent was needed for the use of the public data collected by other researchers.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Bijlagen
Literatuur
go back to reference Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Integrative Guide for the 1991 CBCL/4±18, YSR, and TRF Profiles. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Integrative Guide for the 1991 CBCL/4±18, YSR, and TRF Profiles. University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.
go back to reference Bell, R. Q. (1977). Socialization findings reexamined. In R. Q. Bell & L. V. Harper (Eds.), Child effects on adults (pp. 53–84). Erlbaum. Bell, R. Q. (1977). Socialization findings reexamined. In R. Q. Bell & L. V. Harper (Eds.), Child effects on adults (pp. 53–84). Erlbaum.
go back to reference Enzmann, D.(2018). Die Verbreitung von körperlicher Elterngewalt und Misshandlung im internationalen Vergleich: Ergebnisseder dritten International Self-Report Delinquency (ISRD‑3) Studie [Prevalence of parental physical violence and misuse ininternational comparison: Data from the third International Self-Report Delinquency study]. Rechtspsychologie, 4, 456–475.https://doi.org/10.5771/2365-1083-2018-4-456.CrossRef Enzmann, D.(2018). Die Verbreitung von körperlicher Elterngewalt und Misshandlung im internationalen Vergleich: Ergebnisseder dritten International Self-Report Delinquency (ISRD‑3) Studie [Prevalence of parental physical violence and misuse ininternational comparison: Data from the third International Self-Report Delinquency study]. Rechtspsychologie, 4, 456–475.https://​doi.​org/​10.​5771/​2365-1083-2018-4-456.CrossRef
go back to reference Frick, P. J. (1991). The Alabama Parenting Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, University of Alabama. Frick, P. J. (1991). The Alabama Parenting Questionnaire. Unpublished manuscript, University of Alabama.
go back to reference Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G. J., & Minkow, M. (2010). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind (3rd ed.). McGraw-Hill. Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G. J., & Minkow, M. (2010). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind (3rd ed.). McGraw-Hill.
go back to reference Holden, S. W., & Ashif, R. (2016). Children’s right to safety: The problem of corporal punishment in Pakistan. In S. Deb (ed.). Child safety, welfare and well-being: Issues and challenges (pp. 59–74). Springer. Holden, S. W., & Ashif, R. (2016). Children’s right to safety: The problem of corporal punishment in Pakistan. In S. Deb (ed.). Child safety, welfare and well-being: Issues and challenges (pp. 59–74). Springer.
go back to reference Huntington, S. P. (1996). The clash of civilizations and the remaking of world order. Simon & Schuster. Huntington, S. P. (1996). The clash of civilizations and the remaking of world order. Simon & Schuster.
go back to reference Janson, S., Lanberg, B., & Svensson, B. (2010). Sweden: A 30-year ban on physical punishment of children. In J. E. Durrant & A. B. Smith (eds.), Global pathways to abolishing physical punishment (pp. 241–254). Routledge. Janson, S., Lanberg, B., & Svensson, B. (2010). Sweden: A 30-year ban on physical punishment of children. In J. E. Durrant & A. B. Smith (eds.), Global pathways to abolishing physical punishment (pp. 241–254). Routledge.
go back to reference Lansford, J. E., Chang, L., Dodge, K. A., Malone, P. S., Oburu, P., Palmerus, K., & Quinn, N. (2005). Physical discipline and children’s adjustment: Cultural normativeness as a moderator. Child Development, 76(6), 1234–1246. 10.111/j.1467-8624.2005.00847.x.CrossRef Lansford, J. E., Chang, L., Dodge, K. A., Malone, P. S., Oburu, P., Palmerus, K., & Quinn, N. (2005). Physical discipline and children’s adjustment: Cultural normativeness as a moderator. Child Development, 76(6), 1234–1246. 10.111/j.1467-8624.2005.00847.x.CrossRef
go back to reference Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Sage. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Sage.
go back to reference Manzoni, P., & Schwarzenegger, K. (2019). The influence of earlier parental violence on juvenile delinquency: The role of social bonds, self-control, delinquent peer association and moral values as mediators. European Journal of Criminology & Policy Research, 25, 225–239. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10610-018-9392-3.CrossRef Manzoni, P., & Schwarzenegger, K. (2019). The influence of earlier parental violence on juvenile delinquency: The role of social bonds, self-control, delinquent peer association and moral values as mediators. European Journal of Criminology & Policy Research, 25, 225–239. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10610-018-9392-3.CrossRef
go back to reference Reid, J. B., Patterson, G. R., & Snyder, J. (eds.) (2002). Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental analysis and model for intervention. American Psychological Association. Reid, J. B., Patterson, G. R., & Snyder, J. (eds.) (2002). Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental analysis and model for intervention. American Psychological Association.
go back to reference Rohner, R. P., Bourque, S. L., & Elordi, C. A. (1996). Children’s perceptions of corporal punishment, caretaker acceptance, and psychological adjustment in a poor, biracial southern community. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58(4), 842–852. https://doi.org/10.2307/353974.CrossRef Rohner, R. P., Bourque, S. L., & Elordi, C. A. (1996). Children’s perceptions of corporal punishment, caretaker acceptance, and psychological adjustment in a poor, biracial southern community. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58(4), 842–852. https://​doi.​org/​10.​2307/​353974.CrossRef
go back to reference Spencer, J. E., & Thomas, W. L. (1973). Introducing cultural geography. Wiley. Spencer, J. E., & Thomas, W. L. (1973). Introducing cultural geography. Wiley.
go back to reference Straus, M. (2010). Prevalence, societal causes, and trends in corporal punishment by parents in world perspective. Law and Contemporary Problems, 73(2), 1–30. Straus, M. (2010). Prevalence, societal causes, and trends in corporal punishment by parents in world perspective. Law and Contemporary Problems, 73(2), 1–30.
Metagegevens
Titel
Cultural Differences in the Association of Harsh Parenting with Internalizing and Externalizing Symptoms: A Meta-Analysis
Auteur
Martin Pinquart
Publicatiedatum
08-10-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Child and Family Studies / Uitgave 12/2021
Print ISSN: 1062-1024
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2843
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-021-02113-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 12/2021

Journal of Child and Family Studies 12/2021 Naar de uitgave