Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1/2022

Open Access 12-03-2021 | Brief Communication

Which Factors Influence Teacher Report of Adaptive Functioning in Autistic Children?

Auteurs: Heather L. Moore, Leanne Rogan, Lauren J. Taylor, Tony Charman, Ann Le Couteur, Jonathan Green, Victoria Grahame, the PACT-G Consortium

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 1/2022

share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail
insite
ZOEKEN

Abstract

A wealth of parent-report research shows adaptive functioning difficulties in autistic children, with parent-report influenced by a number of child factors. Adaptive functioning in autistic children is known to vary across settings; however, no research has yet explored factors influencing education professional-report. This study investigated the rate and profile of impairment, and child factors influencing education professional-reported adaptive skills in 248 autistic children. Twelve children were < 3 years (min age for available normative data on the adaptive function measure), so were removed from the analyses. Results replicated parent-literature; adaptive skills were negatively associated with age and informant-reported autism severity, and positively associated with nonverbal ability and expressive language. Adaptive functioning is important for real-world outcomes, e.g. educational attainment, independence, and support needs. Improving our understanding of adaptive functioning in the education context may support opportunities for shared learning and enhance personalised support .
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version of this article (https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10803-021-04930-z) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Collaborators of the PACT-G Consortium are listed in “Acknowledgment” section.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Adaptive functioning describes practical, everyday skills required to meet the demands of the environment. Such difficulties are commonly reported in autistic individuals (Maskey et al. 2012) and link to real-world outcomes such as educational attainment, likelihood of independent living, and requirement for support services (De Bildt et al. 2005; Farley et al. 2009; Taylor and Henninger 2015). Longitudinal follow up of autistic adults shows low rates of independent living, employment, friendships and romantic relationships (Magiati et al. 2014), highlighting the importance of understanding adaptive skills profiles in autistic children for developing personalised support, to improve long term outcomes.
Most research assessing autistic children has used parent-report Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales [VABS; (Sparrow et al. 1984a, b, 2005)], comprising Communication, Daily Living Skills (DLS), Socialisation, Motor Skills, and an overall Adaptive Composite Score (ABC). This informant-report measure utilises information from people who know an individual well and have observed their skills in one or more everyday settings (Merrell 2000), which is especially important for autistic children, who may struggle to communicate, and may lack insight into their own difficulties. Parent-report research broadly reports relative strengths in Motor Skills and relative weaknesses in Socialisation and/or Communication domains (e.g. Nevill et al. 2017; Yang et al. 2016). Significantly lower levels of adaptive functioning are reported by parents of autistic children compared to control groups matched for chronological age (CA) and/or development/IQ, and other neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g. Mouga et al. 2015; Paul et al. 2014; Ventola et al. 2014).
Positive associations are consistently identified between cognitive ability and adaptive functioning in autistic children (Klin et al. 2007; Nevill et al. 2017), although relative to measured IQ, low functioning children show strengths in adaptive ability and high functioning children show weaknesses (e.g. Alvares et al. 2020; Tillmann et al. 2019), weakening the explanatory power of cognitive ability in this group. Few studies have considered language ability independently of cognition; those that did have identified significant positive correlations between language and adaptive skills (e.g. Mayo et al. 2013; Di Rezze et al. 2019). Cross-sectional research indicates a negative relationship between age and adaptive functioning in autistic children (e.g. Klin et al. 2007; McDonald et al. 2015; Nevill et al. 2017). Longitudinal studies, however, reveal a more complex picture of change and stability, influenced by factors such as verbal and nonverbal ability (Farmer et al. 2018; Paynter et al. 2018; Szatmari et al. 2015; Lord et al. 2015). Associations between adaptive functioning and autism severity using direct assessment are variable, with some showing no relationship (Nevill et al. 2017; Ray-Subramanian et al. 2011; Yang et al. 2016), while others identified small but significant negative associations (Paul et al. 2014; Green and Carter 2014; Kanne et al. 2011). In contrast, using parent-report consistently establishes negative relationships (Duncan and Bishop 2015; Liss et al. 2001; McDonald et al. 2015; Perry et al. 2009). Two studies have reported a negative relationship between adaptive functioning and child behaviour problems (Gillham et al. 2000; Green and Carter 2014).
Adaptive functioning in autistic children has been shown to vary over time and across context (Ozonoff et al. 2005; McDonald et al. 2016; De Los Reyes 2011). Thus, it may be helpful to gain additional information from different contexts, for example, in the education setting. To date, little research has explored the factors influencing teacher-reported adaptive functioning in autistic children. Four studies have investigated concordance between parent- and teacher-reported adaptive functioning with varying results (Dickson et al. 2018; Lane et al. 2013; McDonald et al. 2016; Jordan et al. 2019); however, none of these studies investigated which factors influence teacher-report. As adaptive functioning is known to influence educational attainment (De Bildt et al. 2005; Brady et al. 1992), it is important to understand which factors affect teacher-reported adaptive functioning of autistic children.
In order to address this knowledge gap, we used the baseline data of the Paediatric Autism Communication Trial-Generalised (PACT-G; see Green et al. (2018), for details of the trial protocol) to investigate the teacher-report version of the VABS-II (T-VABS-II) in a sample of autistic children. The aims of this study were to determine rates of impairment, compare domain scores for relative strengths and weaknesses in performance, and explore whether any child factors predict reporting of adaptive behaviour on the T-VABS-II. Guided by the parent-report adaptive functioning literature, we hypothesised that:
1.
Children younger than 7 years (sub-scale age cut-off) would show relative strengths in T-VABS-II Motor Skills. Differences in other domains were explored for the whole sample.
 
2.
CA, nonverbal and language ability, and informant-report of autism severity would show a significant relationship with T-VABS-II ABC and domain scores.
 
We also explored the role of teacher-reported behavioural difficulties on T-VABS-II scores.

Methods

Participants

Baseline data were used from 248 children, aged 2–11 years, who were recruited to PACT-G (Green et al. 2018) between November 2016 and April 2018. PACT-G is a randomised controlled trial of a social communication intervention for autistic children. Children in Greater Manchester, the North East of England, and South London were recruited via referral from local clinical and educational services. Eligible children had a clinical diagnosis of autism, which was confirmed using Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2 (ADOS-2; Lord et al. 2012) cut-off scores for ‘Autism’ and Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) scores of ≥ 12 for children < 5 years or ≥ 15 for children ≥ 5 years (Rutter et al. 2003). All children had nonverbal age-equivalent (AE) scores of > 12 months, measured using the Visual Reception (VR) and Fine Motor (FM) subscales of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; Mullen 1995), or the Special Nonverbal Composite Score on the British Ability Scales-School Age (BAS; Elliott and Smith 2011). Children ≥ 5 years were between P3 and P8 on the English curriculum.1 Children with controlled epilepsy were included. Children/parents with significant hearing/visual impairments were excluded, as were parents with severe learning disability or psychiatric disorder. See Table 1 for child characteristics. Parents required enough spoken/written English to participate in PACT-G assessments and intervention. A favourable ethical opinion was obtained from the North West-Greater Manchester Central Research Ethics Committee (REF: 15/NW/0912). Parents provided informed, written consent, and the education provider agreed to participate (see Table 1 for school type).
Table 1
Demographic characteristics of PACT-G sample (N = 248)
 
N (%)
Min
Max
Mean
SD
Child CA (months)
248 (100)
26
131
61.10
22.36
Child Gender
     
 Female
51 (21)
    
 Male
197 (79)
    
Child ethnicity
     
 White-British
136 (55)
    
 White non-British
13 (5)
    
 Mixed/Multiple ethnic backgroundsa
23 (9)
    
 Asian/Asian-British
30 (12)
    
 Black/African/Caribbean/Black British
40 (16)
    
 Other ethnic groupb
6 (2)
    
Type of school
     
 Mainstream nursery
94 (38)
    
 Specialist nursery
5 (2)
    
 Mainstream primary school
50 (20)
    
 Mainstream school with SEN/autism resource class
8 (3)
    
 Special school with mixed disabilities
55 (22)
    
 Specialist autism school
35 (14)
    
 Childminder
1 (1)
    
Phrase speech (ADOS-2 Modulec)
     
 Module 1
187 (75)
    
 Module 2
61 (25)
    
aIncludes mixed white and black Caribbean, white and black African, white and Asian, and any other mixed backgrounds
bIncludes Arab
cADOS-2 module 1: nonverbal-simple phrases e.g. two words); ADOS-2 module 2: short phrases upwards

Measures

Characterisation data from the PACT-G sample used in this study included2:

Adaptive Functioning

The T-VABS-II (Sparrow et al. 2005) is a teacher-assessed questionnaire of adaptive ability for ages 3–21 years. Raw scores were translated into standard scores for Communication, DLS, Socialisation, Motor Skills, and ABC, with a mean of 100 and SD of 15.

Autism Severity

The ADOS-2 (Lord et al. 2012) is a semi-structured, play-based assessment of social communication and restricted and repetitive behaviours. Calibrated Severity Scores (CSS) from 1 to 10 were calculated, which are standardised in relation to CA and verbal ability.
The SCQ Lifetime (Rutter et al. 2003) is a 40-item, parent-report questionnaire that measures social communication behaviours relevant to autism.

Non-verbal Ability

The VR and FM subscales from the MSEL (Mullen 1995) measure nonverbal ability. As our sample included children > 5 years (outside the age range to derive standard scores), we calculated a nonverbal developmental quotient (NVDQ; see Statistical Analysis).

Language Ability

Receptive and Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test (ROWT, EOWT; Martin and Brownell 2011a, b) are picture-based assessments of understanding and use of single words. We used raw scores to capture performance variation of all participants, including those who did not score sufficient correct responses to derive a t-score.

Child Behaviour

The Teacher Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire (T-SDQ; Goodman 1997) measures emotional, conduct, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationships and prosocial behaviour.

Procedure

Assessments administered directly with the child took place at the research clinic, child’s home and/or education setting. Questionnaires were provided to parents and education professionals to complete during or between sessions and return to the research team. Education settings chose the most appropriate person to complete questionnaires, based on prior knowledge of the child. Both parents and education staff were given opportunities to ask questions about any items prior to submission.

Statistical Analysis

Data were prepared and analysed using IBM SPSS Statistics Version 24 (Corp 2016). Twelve participants were removed from the analyses as they were < 3 years, thus younger than the youngest available normative data for the T-VABS-II. Table 2 shows N values for each variable included in the analyses. Teacher-report measures (T-VABS-II and T-SDQ) were defined as missing if education professionals did not return questionnaires or returned them in an incomplete fashion, e.g. T-VABS-II: insufficient subscale items completed to calculate domain scores, or if participants were older than the CA subscale cut-off of 7+ years for T-VABS-II Motor Skills; T-SDQ: < 60% of items completed. Of researcher-administered measures, two participants completed the BAS (Elliott and Smith 2011) so did not have MSEL NVDQ scores. Heightened distress meant it was not possible to complete the ROWT and EOWT with a small number of participants. SDQ subscale scores were prorated according to hand-scoring instructions (Goodman 2001). We used nonparametric equivalents for any data that were not normally distributed.
To determine rates of impairment on T-VABS-II, we examined the percentage of individuals whose ABC and domain standard scores were > 2SD below the normative mean (i.e. < 70). We used a repeated measures ANOVA to compare T-VABS-II domain scores.
Based on findings from the parent VABS literature, we undertook a series of multiple linear regression analyses to investigate whether there were concurrent associations between T-VABS-II ABC/domain scores and child CA, autism severity, nonverbal ability, language ability, and child behaviour, which was supported by correlations between variables in our own data (Supplementary Table 1). All models were examined to ensure that they did not violate the assumptions of linear regression, including multicollinearity (using the VIF). While ADOS-2 CSS did not correlate with T-VABS-II standard scores, we included it to explore prior variable reports regarding direct assessments of autism severity. There were no significant differences between MSEL VR and FM AE scores (Z = − 0.13, p = 0.900; Mean MSEL VR = 27.43 months, Mean MSEL FM = 27.24 months) so we used a mean score to calculate the MSEL NVDQ (nonverbal mental AE/CA*100). Participants performed significantly better on the ROWT than EOWT (Z = − 4.55, p < 0.001); therefore, we entered these predictors separately, rather than deriving a language quotient.

Results

Descriptive statistics (including N) for each measure are presented in Table 2. While the range of T-VABS-II ABC and domain scores showed that some individuals were within AE levels, mean performance in each area was markedly lower than age expectations. Of participants with T-VABS-II ABC scores, 78% scored more than 2SD below the norm. For domain scores, this equated to 69% for Communication, 67% for DLS, 77% for Socialisation, and 50% for Motor Skills. A within subjects ANOVA to compare T-VABS-II domains, using Greenhouse–Geisser correction, indicated significant differences [F(2.78, 503.62) = 13.88, p < 0.001]. Pairwise comparisons showed that participants performed better on the Motor Skills than Communication (p = 0.002), DLS (p = 0.001), and Socialisation (p < 0.001) domains, but no other domains differed significantly. When removing DLS to increase sample size, the nonsignificant difference between other domains remained [F(1.89, 426.06) = 0.38. p = 0.675].
Table 2
Descriptive statistics for measures of autism severity, adaptive functioning, language ability, strengths and difficulties, and nonverbal ability
 
N
Min
Max
Mean
SD
T-VABS-II ABC Standard Score
221
25
98
58.04
14.86
T-VABS-II Communication Standard Score
227
27
114
61.88
16.78
T-VABS-II DLS Standard Score
228
23
108
61.39
16.29
T-VABS-II Socialisation Standard Score
229
23
93
61.43
11.73
T-VABS-II Motor Skills Standard Score
185
38
103
68.61
13.11
ADOS CSS
236
6
10
7.40
1.26
SCQ Total Score
236
12
36
23.58
5.23
MSEL NVDQ
234
12.60
112.24
48.06
18.79
ROWT Total Score
235
0
80
18.75
22.26
EOWT Total Score
232
0
85
15.78
19.23
T-SDQ Total Score
224
5
30
17.51
4.64
Regression analyses for T-VABS-II ABC and domain scores are reported in Table 3. Child CA, parent-report SCQ, and T-SDQ were significant negative predictors of T-VABS-II ABC, while MSEL NVDQ and EOWT were significant positive predictors, accounting for 73% of the variance. Significant negative predictors of T-VABS-II Communication were Child CA, SCQ, and T-SDQ, and significant positive predictors were MSEL NVDQ, ROWT and EOWT, accounting for 77% of the variance. T-VABS-II DLS were significantly negatively associated with Child CA, SCQ, and T-SDQ, but positively associated with MSEL NVDQ and EOWT, accounting for 70% of the variance. Significant negative predictors of T-VABS-II Socialisation were Child CA, SCQ, and T-SDQ, and significant positive predictors were MSEL NVDQ and EOWT, accounting for 58% of the variance. Finally, T-VABS-II Motor Skills scores were square root transformed due to non-normal unstandardised residuals [Shapiro–Wilk(176) = 0.99, p = 0.049]. Motor Skills were significantly negatively associated with Child CA and SCQ, and significantly positively associated with ADOS-2 CSS and MSEL NVDQ, accounting for 29% of the variance.
Table 3
Regression models for T-VABS-II adaptive ability, using child age, autism severity, nonverbal ability, language ability and strengths and difficulties as predictors
 
B (CI)
SE B
β
p
Adjusted r2
T-VABS-II ABC
0.73
 (Constant)
63.38 (50.80, 76.00)
6.38
 
 < .001**
 
 Child CA
− 0.21 (− 0.28, − 0.14)
0.04
− 0.32
 < .001**
 
 ADOS-2 CSS
0.30 (− 0.55, 1.15)
0.43
0.03
.484
 
 SCQ
− 0.26 (− 0.48, − 0.05)
0.11
− 0.09
.015*
 
 MSEL NVDQ
0.26 (0.16, 0.37)
0.05
0.35
 < .001**
 
 ROWT
0.07 (− 0.04, 0.18)
0.06
0.11
.206
 
 EOWT
0.20 (0.09, 0.32)
0.06
0.28
.001**
 
 T-SDQ
− 0.34 (− 0.58, − 0.10)
0.12
− 0.11
.006*
 
T-VABS-II Communication
0.77
 (Constant)
57.80 (44.92, 70.68)
6.53
 
 < .001**
 
 Child CA
− 0.18 (− 0.26, − 0.11)
0.04
− 0.25
 < .001**
 
 ADOS-2 CSS
0.74 (− 0.14, 1.61)
0.44
0.06
.099
 
 SCQ
− 0.24 (− 0.45, − 0.02)
0.11
− 0.08
.035*
 
 MSEL NVDQ
0.31 (0.20, 0.41)
0.05
0.36
 < .001**
 
 ROWT
0.15 (− 0.04, 0.26)
0.06
0.20
.008*
 
 EOWT
0.24 (0.11, 0.36)
0.06
0.28
 < .001**
 
 T-SDQ
− 0.33 (− 0.58, − 0.08)
0.13
− 0.10
.009*
 
T-VABS-II Daily Living Skills
0.70
 (Constant)
79.23 (34.56, 94.01)
7.47
 
 < .001**
 
 Child CA
− 0.28 (− 0.36, − 0.19)
0.04
− 0.38
 < .001**
 
 ADOS-2 CSS
− 0.38 (− 1.37, 0.61)
0.50
− 0.03
.453
 
 SCQ
− 0.32 (− 0.56, − 0.07)
0.13
− 0.10
.013*
 
 MSEL NVDQ
0.31 (0.19, 0.42)
0.06
0.36
 < .001**
 
 ROWT
− 0.02 (− 0.14, 0.11)
0.06
− 0.02
.817
 
 EOWT
0.23 (0.09, 0.36)
0.07
0.27
.001**
 
 T-SDQ
− 0.46 (− 0.74, − 0.18)
0.14
− 0.13
.001**
 
T-VABS-II Socialisation
0.58
 (Constant)
80.63 (68.41, 92.85)
6.20
 
 < .001**
 
 Child CA
− 0.11 (− 0.18, − 0.04)
0.04
− 0.22
.002**
 
 ADOS-2 CSS
− 0.58 (− 1.41, 0.26)
0.42
− 0.06
.173
 
 SCQ
− 0.35 (− 0.56, − 0.15)
0.10
− 0.16
.001**
 
 MSEL NVDQ
0.13 (0.03, 0.23)
0.05
0.22
.009**
 
 ROWT
− 0.03 (− 0.13, 0.08)
0.05
− 0.05
.613
 
 EOWT
0.25 (0.14, 0.37)
0.06
0.43
 < .001**
 
 T-SDQ
− 0.53 (− 0.76, − 0.30)
0.12
− 0.22
 < .001**
 
T-VABS-II Motor Skills (sq root transformed)
0.29
 (Constant)
7.86 (6.63, 9.08
0.62
 
 < .001**
 
 Child CA
− 0.01 (− 0.02, 0.00)
0.01
− 0.18
.039*
 
 ADOS-2 CSS
0.08 (0.03, 0.16)
0.04
0.14
.043*
 
 SCQ
− 0.03 (− 0.05, − 0.01)
0.01
− 0.20
.002**
 
 MSEL NVDQ
0.01 (0.01, 0.02)
0.01
0.33
.004**
 
 ROWT
0.01 (− 0.00, 0.02)
0.01
0.24
.087
 
 EOWT
− 0.00 (− 0.02, 0.01)
0.01
− 0.01
.465
 
 T-SDQ
0.01 (− 0.01, 0.03)
0.01
0.07
.351
 
*Significant at .05 level
**Significant at .01 level

Discussion

As far as the authors are aware, this is the first study to investigate education professional reporting of adaptive functioning in autistic children. We saw high rates of impairment, with at least 50% of children in the ‘low’ range on T-VABS-II ABC/domains (> 2SD below norm). Consistent with our hypotheses, we found relative strengths in Motor Skills, and teacher-report was significantly associated with child factors such as CA, parent-report autism severity, teacher-report behaviour problems, nonverbal and language ability.
Our higher relative T-VABS-II Motor Skills performance is consistent with previous parent-report findings (e.g. Nevill et al. 2017; Yang et al. 2016). In contrast though, we did not find relative weaknesses in T-VABS-II Socialisation or Communication domains. Instead, we found a relatively flat profile with consistent low performance (excluding Motor Skills). This finding may relate to our sampling design, which included older children with lower cognitive abilities that may have negatively impacted adaptive functioning.
Turning to factors that influence T-VABS-II report, nonverbal ability positively predicted ABC/domain scores, in accordance with previous parent-report research (e.g. Perry et al. 2009; Nevill et al. 2017). Unsurprisingly, when looking specifically at the role of language, both receptive and expressive language positively predicted T-VABS-II Communication domain. Additionally, expressive language predicted ABC, DLS and Socialisation. Expressive language may show a stronger relationship to adaptive functioning because many of the T-VABS-II items rely on observation of use or lack of expressive skills, whereas appreciation of language understanding may not be captured as readily. Our results extend previous associations found in the parent literature (e.g. Mayo et al. 2013; Di Rezze et al. 2019) by exploring receptive and expressive language independently.
Child CA and parent-reported autism severity were significant negative predictors of adaptive functioning across all T-VABS-II scores, consistent with parent-report VABS literature (Saulnier and Klin 2007; Klin et al. 2007; McDonald et al. 2015; Nevill et al. 2017). While our data is cross-sectional, so it is not possible to make conclusions about longitudinal, age-related changes, it is likely that autistic children continue to develop their adaptive abilities over time. However, they may not progress at the same rate as typically developing peers, leading to a steady deviation from the age-expected developmental trajectory.
The contribution of direct child autism severity assessment to parent-reported adaptive functioning is inconsistent (e.g. Nevill et al. 2017; Paul et al. 2014), although parent-reported measures have identified significant negative relationships (e.g. Duncan and Bishop 2015; McDonald et al. 2015). However, informant-reported autism severity in parent literature may bias perceptions of adaptive functioning. We dissociated ratings of autism severity (parent SCQ) and adaptive functioning (T-VABS-II) to reduce this risk of observer bias, and identified negative associations with adaptive functioning. There was no relationship with direct assessment of autism severity (ADOS-2), which could be expected given the variable parent literature, except for a positive association with Motor Skills. This relationship was unexpected, especially given the high prevalence of motor difficulties in autism (Green et al. 2009), and further research is needed to elucidate this finding.
These differences might be explained by the properties of the measures. The ADOS-2 is undertaken during a set period and as such, may not capture all autism-related features across different contexts that are more broadly relevant to adaptive behaviour. Further, the ADOS-2 CSS may not necessarily be sensitive enough to tease out differences in autism severity between individuals. Conversely, items on the SCQ may overlap with the T-VABS-II, creating stronger associations as a result. Informant measures tend also to record the most severe behaviour exhibited by a child, so in some cases, these scores may represent early deficits or extremes of behaviour rather than current symptoms or general presentation. It is possible, though, that these early deficits may have a lasting impact on development of adaptive abilities, explaining the variability between these two types of measure.
Finally, our study found reduced adaptive functioning with increasing T-SDQ difficulties in all T-VABS-II scores except Motor Skills, supporting previous findings (Gillham et al. 2000; Green and Carter 2014), although some variance may be explained by the similarity in themes of items on both measures. Autistic children have high rates of emotional and behavioural problems (Maskey et al. 2012; Chandler et al. 2016); thus, it is particularly relevant to explore further how this impacts on everyday functioning.
Our results show that similar child characteristics influence education professional and parent report of adaptive functioning (as identified extensively in previous parent literature). Children with lower adaptive abilities require greater special education provision (De Bildt et al. 2005), suggesting this may be an important area of focus in education. However, autistic children can have difficulties generalising learnt behaviours across different contexts (McDonald et al. 2016); thus, differences may arise in parent and education professional reports of adaptive functioning. While a small number of studies have shown moderate agreement in parent- and teacher-reported domain scores (Dickson et al. 2018; Lane et al. 2013; McDonald et al. 2016; Jordan et al. 2019), no study to date has examined whether this is driven by consistent responses to individual behaviours within each domain. To better understand adaptive functioning across contexts, we plan to undertake a detailed comparison of parent and teacher VABS-II item level responses (Moore et al. in preparation).
Our results are potentially limited by lack of knowledge about the background and experience of the education professionals who completed our questionnaires. Informants differed by level of education and teaching experience, as well as autism training and experience, type of education environment, class size and potentially, familiarity with the child. The T-VABS-II assumes knowledge about developmental levels and terminology that may vary based on informant characteristics. It is not possible for the researchers to understand whether informants used standard thresholds for scoring within each item in this study, but in future, it would be beneficial to capture more detailed information about informant characteristics and factor these into analyses, to determine any impact on reporting.
Our findings indicate real adaptive functioning difficulties as reported by education professionals, and influences of CA, informant-reported autism severity, nonverbal ability and expressive language on reporting. Adaptive skills are important for real-world outcomes of autistic individuals, including educational attainment, level of support needs, and independence, making them an important educational focus. Gaining further understanding of the relationships between adaptive skills and other skills may enhance opportunities for shared learning and inform personalised support, to improve long term outcomes.

Acknowledgments

The Paediatric Autism Communication Trial-Generalised (PACT-G) is funded by the National Institute of Health Research (NIHR) and Medical Research Council [Efficacy and Mechanism Evaluation Programme (13/119/18)]. The views expressed here are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care. We would like to express our gratitude for the valued contribution of all the participating children, families, nurseries, schools and referring clinicians. We acknowledge the contribution to data collection of Alana Barnett, Lydia Johnson-Ferguson, Rachel Lisle, Hafsa Rahman, Hafiza Sadiq.

Collaborators for the The PACT-G Consortium

Catherine Aldred (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Matea Balabanovska (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Hilary Beach (Manchester University NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester, UK), Claire Bennett (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Sophie Carruthers (King’s College, London, UK), Imogen Crook (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Hannah Danvers (Manchester University NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester, UK), Kate Dartnall (Manchester University NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester, UK), Ceri Ellis (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Richard Emsley (King’s College, London, UK), Hannah Foote (Guy’s & St Thomas’ NHS Trust, London, UK), Jessica Graham (Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Patricia Howlin (King’s College, London, UK), Neil Humphrey (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Kirsty James (King’s College, London, UK), Sarah Jamieson (Cumbria, Northumberland, Tyne & Wear NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Anna Kappa (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Anna Knight (Cumbria, Northumberland, Tyne & Wear NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Kathy Leadbitter (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Jo Lowe (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Sophie Langhorne (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Ruth Madeley (Cumbria, Northumberland, Tyne & Wear NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Deborah Maskell (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Olivia Mitchell (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Helen McConachie (Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Francisca Monteiro (King’s College, London, UK), Cat Papastavrou Brooks (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Jeremy Parr (Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK; (Cumbria, Northumberland, Tyne & Wear NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK)) , Amelia Pearson (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Andrew Pickles (King’s College, London, UK), Jessica Rose (Guy’s & St Thomas’ NHS Trust, London, UK), Lisa Slater (Cumbria, Northumberland, Tyne & Wear NHS Foundation Trust, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Vicky Slonims (Guy’s & St Thomas’ NHS Trust, London, UK), Carol Taylor (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK), Susanna Vosper (Manchester University NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester, UK), Helen Wilson (University of Manchester, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK).

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Ethical Approval

A favourable ethical opinion was obtained from the North West-Greater Manchester Central Research Ethics Committee (REF: 15/NW/0912). Parents provided informed, written consent, and the child’s education provider agreed to participate.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Voetnoten
1
In England, P scales describe targets for children aged 5–16 years with special educational needs. P8 was taken to represent a language age-equivalent of approximately 4 years in a typically developing child.
 
2
Data from selected measures were used for the purpose of this study. For complete baseline characterisation data of the PACT-G sample, see Green et al. (2018).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Corp, I. (2016). IBM SPSS statistics for windows (24th ed.). Armonk, NY: IBM Corp. Corp, I. (2016). IBM SPSS statistics for windows (24th ed.). Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.
go back to reference De Los Reyes, A. (2011). Introduction to the special section: More than measurement error: Discovering meaning behind informant discrepancies in clinical assessments of children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, 1–9.CrossRef De Los Reyes, A. (2011). Introduction to the special section: More than measurement error: Discovering meaning behind informant discrepancies in clinical assessments of children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, 1–9.CrossRef
go back to reference Dickson, K. S., Suhrheinrich, J., Rieth, S. R., & Stahmer, A. C. (2018). Parent and teacher concordance of child outcomes for youth with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(5), 1423–1435.CrossRef Dickson, K. S., Suhrheinrich, J., Rieth, S. R., & Stahmer, A. C. (2018). Parent and teacher concordance of child outcomes for youth with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(5), 1423–1435.CrossRef
go back to reference Duncan, A. W., & Bishop, S. L. (2015). Understanding the gap between cognitive abilities and daily living skills in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders with average intelligence. Autism, 19(1), 64–72.CrossRef Duncan, A. W., & Bishop, S. L. (2015). Understanding the gap between cognitive abilities and daily living skills in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders with average intelligence. Autism, 19(1), 64–72.CrossRef
go back to reference Elliott, C. D., & Smith, P. (2011). British ability scales-3 technical manual. London: GL Assessment Limited. Elliott, C. D., & Smith, P. (2011). British ability scales-3 technical manual. London: GL Assessment Limited.
go back to reference Gillham, J. E., Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (2000). Toward a developmental operational definition of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(4), 269–278.CrossRef Gillham, J. E., Carter, A. S., Volkmar, F. R., & Sparrow, S. S. (2000). Toward a developmental operational definition of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(4), 269–278.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581–586.CrossRef Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581–586.CrossRef
go back to reference Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 1337–1345.CrossRef Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 1337–1345.CrossRef
go back to reference Green, S. A., & Carter, A. S. (2014). Predictors and course of daily living skills development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(2), 256–263.CrossRef Green, S. A., & Carter, A. S. (2014). Predictors and course of daily living skills development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(2), 256–263.CrossRef
go back to reference Kanne, S. M., Gerber, A. J., Quirmbach, L. M., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Saulnier, C. A. (2011). The role of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Implications for functional outcome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 1007–1018.CrossRef Kanne, S. M., Gerber, A. J., Quirmbach, L. M., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Saulnier, C. A. (2011). The role of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Implications for functional outcome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 1007–1018.CrossRef
go back to reference Klin, A., Saulnier, C. A., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., Volkmar, F. R., & Lord, C. (2007). Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: The Vineland and the ADOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 748–759.CrossRef Klin, A., Saulnier, C. A., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., Volkmar, F. R., & Lord, C. (2007). Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: The Vineland and the ADOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(4), 748–759.CrossRef
go back to reference Lane, B. R., Paynter, J., & Sharman, R. (2013). Parent and teacher ratings of adaptive and challenging behaviours in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(10), 1196–1203.CrossRef Lane, B. R., Paynter, J., & Sharman, R. (2013). Parent and teacher ratings of adaptive and challenging behaviours in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(10), 1196–1203.CrossRef
go back to reference Liss, M., Harel, B., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., et al. (2001). Predictors and correlates of adaptive functioning in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(2), 219–230.CrossRef Liss, M., Harel, B., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., et al. (2001). Predictors and correlates of adaptive functioning in children with developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(2), 219–230.CrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule (2nd ed.). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism diagnostic observation schedule (2nd ed.). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Martin, N., & Brownell, R. (2011a). Expressive one word picture vocabulary test (4th ed.). Novato, CA: ATP Assessments. Martin, N., & Brownell, R. (2011a). Expressive one word picture vocabulary test (4th ed.). Novato, CA: ATP Assessments.
go back to reference Martin, N., & Brownell, R. (2011b). Receptive one word picture vocabulary test (4th ed.). Novato, CA: ATP Assessments. Martin, N., & Brownell, R. (2011b). Receptive one word picture vocabulary test (4th ed.). Novato, CA: ATP Assessments.
go back to reference McDonald, C. A., Thomeer, M. L., Lopata, C., Fox, J. D., Donnelly, J. P., Tang, V., et al. (2015). VABS-II ratings and predictors of adaptive behavior in children with HFASD. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 27(2), 235–247.CrossRef McDonald, C. A., Thomeer, M. L., Lopata, C., Fox, J. D., Donnelly, J. P., Tang, V., et al. (2015). VABS-II ratings and predictors of adaptive behavior in children with HFASD. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 27(2), 235–247.CrossRef
go back to reference McDonald, C. A., Lopata, C., Donnelly, J. P., Thomeer, M. L., Rodgers, J. D., & Jordan, A. K. (2016). Informant discrepancies in externalizing and internalizing symptoms and adaptive skills of high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder. School Psychology Quarterly, 31(4), 467–477.CrossRef McDonald, C. A., Lopata, C., Donnelly, J. P., Thomeer, M. L., Rodgers, J. D., & Jordan, A. K. (2016). Informant discrepancies in externalizing and internalizing symptoms and adaptive skills of high-functioning children with autism spectrum disorder. School Psychology Quarterly, 31(4), 467–477.CrossRef
go back to reference Merrell, K. W. (2000). Informant report: Rating Scale measures. In E. S. Shapiro & T. R. Kratochwill (Eds.), Conducting school-based assessments of child and adolescent behavior (pp. 203–234). New York: The Guildford Press. Merrell, K. W. (2000). Informant report: Rating Scale measures. In E. S. Shapiro & T. R. Kratochwill (Eds.), Conducting school-based assessments of child and adolescent behavior (pp. 203–234). New York: The Guildford Press.
go back to reference Mouga, S., Almeida, J., Café, C., Duque, F., & Oliveira, G. (2015). Adaptive profiles in autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(4), 1001–1012.CrossRef Mouga, S., Almeida, J., Café, C., Duque, F., & Oliveira, G. (2015). Adaptive profiles in autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(4), 1001–1012.CrossRef
go back to reference Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: Western Psychological Services. Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning. Circle Pines, MN: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Nevill, R., Hedley, D., Uljarevic, M., & Mulick, J. A. (2017). Adaptive behavior profiles in young children with autism spectrum disorder diagnosed under DSM-5 criteria. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 43–44, 53–66.CrossRef Nevill, R., Hedley, D., Uljarevic, M., & Mulick, J. A. (2017). Adaptive behavior profiles in young children with autism spectrum disorder diagnosed under DSM-5 criteria. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 43–44, 53–66.CrossRef
go back to reference Ozonoff, S., Goodlin-Jones, B., & Solomon, M. (2005). Evidence-based assesment of autism spectrum disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 34(3), 559–568.CrossRef Ozonoff, S., Goodlin-Jones, B., & Solomon, M. (2005). Evidence-based assesment of autism spectrum disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 34(3), 559–568.CrossRef
go back to reference Paul, R., Loomis, R., & Chawarska, K. (2014). Adaptive behavior in toddlers under two with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(2), 264–270.CrossRef Paul, R., Loomis, R., & Chawarska, K. (2014). Adaptive behavior in toddlers under two with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(2), 264–270.CrossRef
go back to reference Perry, A., Flanagan, H. E., Dunn Geier, J., & Freeman, N. L. (2009). Brief report: The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales in young children with autism spectrum disorders at different cognitive levels. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(7), 1066–1078.CrossRef Perry, A., Flanagan, H. E., Dunn Geier, J., & Freeman, N. L. (2009). Brief report: The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales in young children with autism spectrum disorders at different cognitive levels. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(7), 1066–1078.CrossRef
go back to reference Ray-Subramanian, C. E., Huai, N., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2011). Brief report: adaptive behavior and cognitive skills for toddlers on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 679–684.CrossRef Ray-Subramanian, C. E., Huai, N., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2011). Brief report: adaptive behavior and cognitive skills for toddlers on the autism spectrum. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(5), 679–684.CrossRef
go back to reference Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles, VA: Western Psychological Services. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles, VA: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Saulnier, C. A., & Klin, A. (2007). Brief Report: Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism and asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 788–793.CrossRef Saulnier, C. A., & Klin, A. (2007). Brief Report: Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism and asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 788–793.CrossRef
go back to reference Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. A. (1984a). Vineland adaptive behavior scales (expanded form). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. A. (1984a). Vineland adaptive behavior scales (expanded form). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
go back to reference Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. A. (1984b). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (survey form). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. A. (1984b). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (survey form). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
go back to reference Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland-II: Vineland adaptive behaviour scales, survey forms manual. Circle Pines, MN: Pearson. Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland-II: Vineland adaptive behaviour scales, survey forms manual. Circle Pines, MN: Pearson.
go back to reference Ventola, P., Saulnier, C. A., Steinberg, E., Katarzyna Chawarska, K., & Klin, A. (2014). Early-emerging social adaptive skills in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: An item analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 283–293.CrossRef Ventola, P., Saulnier, C. A., Steinberg, E., Katarzyna Chawarska, K., & Klin, A. (2014). Early-emerging social adaptive skills in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: An item analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 283–293.CrossRef
go back to reference Yang, S., Paynter, J. M., & Gilmore, L. (2016). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: II profile of young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(1), 64–73.CrossRef Yang, S., Paynter, J. M., & Gilmore, L. (2016). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: II profile of young children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(1), 64–73.CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Which Factors Influence Teacher Report of Adaptive Functioning in Autistic Children?
Auteurs
Heather L. Moore
Leanne Rogan
Lauren J. Taylor
Tony Charman
Ann Le Couteur
Jonathan Green
Victoria Grahame
the PACT-G Consortium
Publicatiedatum
12-03-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 1/2022
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-04930-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2022

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1/2022 Naar de uitgave