Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 7/2019

29-04-2019 | Brief Report

Brief Report: Visual Perception, Task-Induced Pupil Response Trajectories and ASD Features in Children

Auteurs: Antoinette Sabatino DiCriscio, Yirui Hu, Vanessa Troiani

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 7/2019

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

We applied a trajectory-based analysis to eye tracking data in order to quantify individualized patterns of pupil response in the context of global–local processing that may be associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) features. Multiple pupil response trajectories across both global and local conditions were identified. Using the combined trajectory patterns for global and local conditions for each individual, we were able to identify three groups based on trajectory group membership that were thought to reflect perceptual strategy. Results indicated that the proportion of children with ASD was significantly greater in the group demonstrating a local-focus response. This research presents a novel analytic approach to the objective characterization of individualized pupil response patterns that are associated with ASD features.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
While stimuli were presented for 5.5 s, due to consistent variability in Tobii eyetracking acquisitions (some having slightly over and/or under 330 samples taken across each 5.5 stimulus presentation) the initial 300 collected samples within each trial were used in analyses.
 
2
We also explored potential differences in baseline pupil diameter between global and local conditions between our pupil trajectory groups; however, results did not indicate significant differences in baseline pupil diameter associated with pupil trajectory groups (p’s > 0.73).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Ahissar, M., & Hochstein, S. (2004). The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(10), 457–464.CrossRef Ahissar, M., & Hochstein, S. (2004). The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(10), 457–464.CrossRef
go back to reference Ashwin, E., Ashwin, C., Rhydderch, D., Howells, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2009a). Eagle-eyed visual acuity: An experimental investigation of enhanced perception in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 65(1), 17–21.CrossRef Ashwin, E., Ashwin, C., Rhydderch, D., Howells, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2009a). Eagle-eyed visual acuity: An experimental investigation of enhanced perception in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 65(1), 17–21.CrossRef
go back to reference Ashwin, E., Ashwin, C., Tavassoli, T., Chakrabarti, B., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2009b). Eagle-eyed visual acuity in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 66(10), e23–e24.CrossRef Ashwin, E., Ashwin, C., Tavassoli, T., Chakrabarti, B., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2009b). Eagle-eyed visual acuity in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 66(10), e23–e24.CrossRef
go back to reference Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.CrossRef Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390–412.CrossRef
go back to reference Bach, M., & Dakin, S. C. (2009). Regarding “Eagle-eyed visual acuity: An experimental investigation of enhanced perception in autism”. Biological Psychiatry, 66(10), e19–e20.CrossRef Bach, M., & Dakin, S. C. (2009). Regarding “Eagle-eyed visual acuity: An experimental investigation of enhanced perception in autism”. Biological Psychiatry, 66(10), e19–e20.CrossRef
go back to reference Binda, P., Pereverzeva, M., & Murray, S. O. (2013). Attention to bright surfaces enhances the pupillary light reflex. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(5), 2199–2204.CrossRef Binda, P., Pereverzeva, M., & Murray, S. O. (2013). Attention to bright surfaces enhances the pupillary light reflex. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(5), 2199–2204.CrossRef
go back to reference Boev, A. N., Fountas, K. N., Karampelas, I., Boev, C., Machinis, T. G., Feltes, C., et al. (2005). Quantitative pupillometry: Normative data in healthy pediatric volunteers. Journal of Neurosurgery: Pediatrics, 103(6), 496–500. Boev, A. N., Fountas, K. N., Karampelas, I., Boev, C., Machinis, T. G., Feltes, C., et al. (2005). Quantitative pupillometry: Normative data in healthy pediatric volunteers. Journal of Neurosurgery: Pediatrics, 103(6), 496–500.
go back to reference Bölte, S., Schlitt, S., Gapp, V., Hainz, D., Schirman, S., Poustka, F., et al. (2012). A close eye on the eagle-eyed visual acuity hypothesis of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(5), 726–733.CrossRef Bölte, S., Schlitt, S., Gapp, V., Hainz, D., Schirman, S., Poustka, F., et al. (2012). A close eye on the eagle-eyed visual acuity hypothesis of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(5), 726–733.CrossRef
go back to reference Busch, N. A., Dubois, J., & VanRullen, R. (2009). The phase of ongoing EEG oscillations predicts visual perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(24), 7869–7876.CrossRef Busch, N. A., Dubois, J., & VanRullen, R. (2009). The phase of ongoing EEG oscillations predicts visual perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(24), 7869–7876.CrossRef
go back to reference Constantino, J. N., Davis, S. A., Todd, R. D., Schindler, M. K., Gross, M. M., Brophy, S. L., et al. (2003). Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: Comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 427–433.CrossRef Constantino, J. N., Davis, S. A., Todd, R. D., Schindler, M. K., Gross, M. M., Brophy, S. L., et al. (2003). Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: Comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 427–433.CrossRef
go back to reference Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic traits in the general population: A twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(5), 524–530.CrossRef Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic traits in the general population: A twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(5), 524–530.CrossRef
go back to reference Crewther, D. P., & Sutherland, A. (2009). The more he looked inside, the more piglet wasn’t there: Is autism really blessed with visual hyperacuity? Biological Psychiatry, 66(10), e21–e22.CrossRef Crewther, D. P., & Sutherland, A. (2009). The more he looked inside, the more piglet wasn’t there: Is autism really blessed with visual hyperacuity? Biological Psychiatry, 66(10), e21–e22.CrossRef
go back to reference D’Souza, D., Booth, R., Connolly, M., Happé, F., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2016). Rethinking the concepts of ‘local or global processors’: Evidence from Williams syndrome, Down syndrome, and Autism Spectrum Disorders. Developmental Science, 19(3), 452–468.CrossRef D’Souza, D., Booth, R., Connolly, M., Happé, F., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2016). Rethinking the concepts of ‘local or global processors’: Evidence from Williams syndrome, Down syndrome, and Autism Spectrum Disorders. Developmental Science, 19(3), 452–468.CrossRef
go back to reference DiCriscio, A. S., Hu, Y., & Troiani, V. (2018). Task induced pupil response and visual perception in adults. PLoS ONE, 13(12), e0209556.CrossRef DiCriscio, A. S., Hu, Y., & Troiani, V. (2018). Task induced pupil response and visual perception in adults. PLoS ONE, 13(12), e0209556.CrossRef
go back to reference DiCriscio, A. S., Miller, S. J., Hanna, E. K., Kovac, M., Turner-Brown, L., Sasson, N. J., et al. (2016). Brief report: Cognitive control of social and nonsocial visual attention in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(8), 2797–2805.CrossRef DiCriscio, A. S., Miller, S. J., Hanna, E. K., Kovac, M., Turner-Brown, L., Sasson, N. J., et al. (2016). Brief report: Cognitive control of social and nonsocial visual attention in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(8), 2797–2805.CrossRef
go back to reference DiCriscio, A. S., & Troiani, V. (2017). Pupil adaptation corresponds to quantitative measures of autism traits in children. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 6476.CrossRef DiCriscio, A. S., & Troiani, V. (2017). Pupil adaptation corresponds to quantitative measures of autism traits in children. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 6476.CrossRef
go back to reference Dukette, D., & Stiles, J. (2001). The effects of stimulus density on children’s analysis of hierarchical patterns. Developmental Science, 4(2), 233–251.CrossRef Dukette, D., & Stiles, J. (2001). The effects of stimulus density on children’s analysis of hierarchical patterns. Developmental Science, 4(2), 233–251.CrossRef
go back to reference Happe, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Science, 3(6), 216–222.CrossRef Happe, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Science, 3(6), 216–222.CrossRef
go back to reference Hayward, D. A., Fenerci, C., & Ristic, J. (2018). An investigation of global–local processing bias in a large sample of typical individuals varying in autism traits. Consciousness and Cognition, 65, 271–279.CrossRef Hayward, D. A., Fenerci, C., & Ristic, J. (2018). An investigation of global–local processing bias in a large sample of typical individuals varying in autism traits. Consciousness and Cognition, 65, 271–279.CrossRef
go back to reference Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Brown, R. (1999). Autism and congenital blindness. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(1), 45–56.CrossRef Hobson, R. P., Lee, A., & Brown, R. (1999). Autism and congenital blindness. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(1), 45–56.CrossRef
go back to reference Hochstein, S., & Ahissar, M. (2002). View from the top: Hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system. Neuron, 36(5), 791–804.CrossRef Hochstein, S., & Ahissar, M. (2002). View from the top: Hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system. Neuron, 36(5), 791–804.CrossRef
go back to reference Jackson, I., & Sirois, S. (2009). Infant cognition: Going full factorial with pupil dilation. Developmental Science, 12(4), 670–679.CrossRef Jackson, I., & Sirois, S. (2009). Infant cognition: Going full factorial with pupil dilation. Developmental Science, 12(4), 670–679.CrossRef
go back to reference Jepma, M., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2011). Pupil diameter predicts changes in the exploration–exploitation trade-off: Evidence for the adaptive gain theory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(7), 1587–1596.CrossRef Jepma, M., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2011). Pupil diameter predicts changes in the exploration–exploitation trade-off: Evidence for the adaptive gain theory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(7), 1587–1596.CrossRef
go back to reference Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., Weigelt, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2013). Global/local processing in autism: Not a disability, but a disinclination. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(10), 2329–2340.CrossRef Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., Weigelt, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2013). Global/local processing in autism: Not a disability, but a disinclination. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(10), 2329–2340.CrossRef
go back to reference Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2003). Weak coherence, no theory of mind, or executive dysfunction? Solving the puzzle of pragmatic language disorders. Brain and Language, 85(3), 451–466.CrossRef Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2003). Weak coherence, no theory of mind, or executive dysfunction? Solving the puzzle of pragmatic language disorders. Brain and Language, 85(3), 451–466.CrossRef
go back to reference McKone, E., Davies, A. A., Fernando, D., Aalders, R., Leung, H., Wickramariyaratne, T., et al. (2010). Asia has the global advantage: Race and visual attention. Vision Research, 50(16), 1540–1549.CrossRef McKone, E., Davies, A. A., Fernando, D., Aalders, R., Leung, H., Wickramariyaratne, T., et al. (2010). Asia has the global advantage: Race and visual attention. Vision Research, 50(16), 1540–1549.CrossRef
go back to reference Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Stauder, J. E. A., & Robaey, P. (1999). Perceptual processing among high-functioning persons with autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(02), 203–211.CrossRef Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Stauder, J. E. A., & Robaey, P. (1999). Perceptual processing among high-functioning persons with autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(02), 203–211.CrossRef
go back to reference Naber, M., Alvarez, G. A., & Nakayama, K. (2013). Tracking the allocation of attention using human pupillary oscillations. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 919.CrossRef Naber, M., Alvarez, G. A., & Nakayama, K. (2013). Tracking the allocation of attention using human pupillary oscillations. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 919.CrossRef
go back to reference Nagin, D. S., & Nagin, D. (2005). Group-based modeling of development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRef Nagin, D. S., & Nagin, D. (2005). Group-based modeling of development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353–383.CrossRef Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9(3), 353–383.CrossRef
go back to reference Navon, D. (1981). The forest revisited: More on global precedence. Psychological Research, 43(1), 1–32.CrossRef Navon, D. (1981). The forest revisited: More on global precedence. Psychological Research, 43(1), 1–32.CrossRef
go back to reference Palinko, O., Kun, A. L., Shyrokov, A., & Heeman, P. (2010). Estimating cognitive load using remote eye tracking in a driving simulator. In Proceedings of the 2010 symposium on eye-tracking research & applications (pp. 141–144). New York: ACM. Palinko, O., Kun, A. L., Shyrokov, A., & Heeman, P. (2010). Estimating cognitive load using remote eye tracking in a driving simulator. In Proceedings of the 2010 symposium on eye-tracking research & applications (pp. 141–144). New York: ACM.
go back to reference Pellicano, E., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Maley, A. (2006). Multiple cognitive capabilities/deficits in children with an autism spectrum disorder: “Weak” central coherence and its relationship to theory of mind and executive control. Development and Psychopathology, 18(1), 77–98.CrossRef Pellicano, E., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Maley, A. (2006). Multiple cognitive capabilities/deficits in children with an autism spectrum disorder: “Weak” central coherence and its relationship to theory of mind and executive control. Development and Psychopathology, 18(1), 77–98.CrossRef
go back to reference Poirel, N., Mellet, E., Houdé, O., & Pineau, A. (2008). First came the trees, then the forest: Developmental changes during childhood in the processing of visual local–global patterns according to the meaningfulness of the stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 245.CrossRef Poirel, N., Mellet, E., Houdé, O., & Pineau, A. (2008). First came the trees, then the forest: Developmental changes during childhood in the processing of visual local–global patterns according to the meaningfulness of the stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 245.CrossRef
go back to reference Poirel, N., Pineau, A., & Mellet, E. (2006). Implicit identification of irrelevant local objects interacts with global/local processing of hierarchical stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 122(3), 321–336.CrossRef Poirel, N., Pineau, A., & Mellet, E. (2006). Implicit identification of irrelevant local objects interacts with global/local processing of hierarchical stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 122(3), 321–336.CrossRef
go back to reference Poirel, N., Simon, G., Cassotti, M., Leroux, G., Perchey, G., Lanoë, C., et al. (2011). The shift from local to global visual processing in 6-year-old children is associated with grey matter loss. PLoS ONE, 6(6), e20879.CrossRef Poirel, N., Simon, G., Cassotti, M., Leroux, G., Perchey, G., Lanoë, C., et al. (2011). The shift from local to global visual processing in 6-year-old children is associated with grey matter loss. PLoS ONE, 6(6), e20879.CrossRef
go back to reference Raney, G. E., Campbell, S. J., & Bovee, J. C. (2014). Using eye movements to evaluate the cognitive processes involved in text comprehension. Journal of Visualized Experiments: JoVE, 10(83), e50780. Raney, G. E., Campbell, S. J., & Bovee, J. C. (2014). Using eye movements to evaluate the cognitive processes involved in text comprehension. Journal of Visualized Experiments: JoVE, 10(83), e50780.
go back to reference Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Holtzclaw, T. N., Lam, K. S. L., & Bodfish, J. W. (2008). Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention during passive viewing of complex social and nonsocial picture arrays. Autism Research, 1(1), 31–42. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.4.CrossRefPubMed Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Holtzclaw, T. N., Lam, K. S. L., & Bodfish, J. W. (2008). Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention during passive viewing of complex social and nonsocial picture arrays. Autism Research, 1(1), 31–42. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1002/​aur.​4.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Scherf, K. S., Behrmann, M., Kimchi, R., & Luna, B. (2009). Emergence of global shape processing continues through adolescence. Child Development, 80(1), 162–177.CrossRef Scherf, K. S., Behrmann, M., Kimchi, R., & Luna, B. (2009). Emergence of global shape processing continues through adolescence. Child Development, 80(1), 162–177.CrossRef
go back to reference Stiles, J., & Tada, W. L. (1996). Developmental change in children’s analysis of spatial patterns. Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 951.CrossRef Stiles, J., & Tada, W. L. (1996). Developmental change in children’s analysis of spatial patterns. Developmental Psychology, 32(5), 951.CrossRef
go back to reference Varnum, M. E., Shi, Z., Chen, A., Qiu, J., & Han, S. (2014). When “Your” reward is the same as “My” reward: Self-construal priming shifts neural responses to own vs. friends’ rewards. NeuroImage, 87, 164–169.CrossRef Varnum, M. E., Shi, Z., Chen, A., Qiu, J., & Han, S. (2014). When “Your” reward is the same as “My” reward: Self-construal priming shifts neural responses to own vs. friends’ rewards. NeuroImage, 87, 164–169.CrossRef
go back to reference Wechsler, D., & Hsiao-pin, C. (2011). WASI-II: Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. London: Pearson. Wechsler, D., & Hsiao-pin, C. (2011). WASI-II: Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. London: Pearson.
Metagegevens
Titel
Brief Report: Visual Perception, Task-Induced Pupil Response Trajectories and ASD Features in Children
Auteurs
Antoinette Sabatino DiCriscio
Yirui Hu
Vanessa Troiani
Publicatiedatum
29-04-2019
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 7/2019
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04028-7

Andere artikelen Uitgave 7/2019

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 7/2019 Naar de uitgave