Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1/2015

01-01-2015 | Original Paper

Symbolic Understanding of Pictures in Low-Functioning Children with Autism: The Effects of Iconicity and Naming

Auteurs: Calum Hartley, Melissa L. Allen

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 1/2015

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This research investigated whether symbolic understanding of pictures in low-functioning children with autism is mediated by iconicity and language. In Experiment 1, participants were taught novel words paired with unfamiliar pictures that varied in iconicity (black-and-white line drawings, greyscale photographs, colour line drawings, colour photographs). Unlike mental-age matched typically developing peers, children with autism generally mapped words onto pictures rather than depicted referents, however, they generalised labels more frequently in colour picture conditions. In Experiment 2, children with autism categorised a line drawing with its referent, rather than another picture, regardless of whether it was named. Typically developing children only viewed pictures as symbols when they were labelled. Overall, symbolic understanding of pictures in children with autism is facilitated by iconicity (particularly colour), but not language.
Voetnoten
1
Ten children with autism achieved scores that had standardised age equivalents (M: ability 3.8 years, SD: 1.1) which were individually matched to those of typically developing children (M: ability 3.7 years, SD: 1). The BPVS scores of three children with autism who completed the measure did not have age equivalents (they were too low), and so they were matched to three typically developing children based on their raw scores. Another seven children with autism were unable to complete the BPVS due to behavioural issues, thus these participants were matched to seven typically developing children who each had a receptive language age of approximately three-years. Although it would have been desirable to acquire a measure of receptive language from every child with autism, it is important to note that this research is particularly relevant to nonverbal individuals who have difficulty with standardised assessments, display challenging behaviours and receive picture-based communication training.
 
2
Group means replaced the missing values of the two children with autism who did not complete colour photograph and greyscale photograph trials, respectively.
 
3
As stated previously, some children with autism did not have BPVS scores. In order to assess the relative language abilities of children with autism in the Labelled and Unlabelled conditions, we conservatively assigned participants without a BPVS score a receptive vocabulary age of 2.5 years as this is the youngest age at which typically developing children can complete the task (which all children with autism managed).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental health disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental health disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68, 48–57. doi:10.2307/1131924.PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A., & Crowson, M. (1997). Do children with autism use the speaker’s direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Child Development, 68, 48–57. doi:10.​2307/​1131924.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meanings of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Bloomberg, K., Karlan, G. R., & Lloyd, L. L. (1990). The comparative translucency of initial lexical items represented in five graphic symbol systems and sets. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 717–725.PubMedCrossRef Bloomberg, K., Karlan, G. R., & Lloyd, L. L. (1990). The comparative translucency of initial lexical items represented in five graphic symbol systems and sets. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 717–725.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bondy, A., & Frost, L. (1994). The picture exchange communication system. Focus on Autistic Behavior, 9, 1–19. Bondy, A., & Frost, L. (1994). The picture exchange communication system. Focus on Autistic Behavior, 9, 1–19.
go back to reference Callaghan, T. C. (2008). The origins and development of pictorial symbolic functioning. In C. Milbrath & H. Trautner (Eds.), Children’s understanding and production of pictures, drawings, and art (pp. 21–32). Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe and Huber. Callaghan, T. C. (2008). The origins and development of pictorial symbolic functioning. In C. Milbrath & H. Trautner (Eds.), Children’s understanding and production of pictures, drawings, and art (pp. 21–32). Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe and Huber.
go back to reference Callaghan, T. C., & Rochat, P. (2008). Children’s understanding of artist-picture relations: Implications for their theories of pictures. In C. Milbrath & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), Children’s understanding and production of pictures, drawings, and art (pp. 186–205). Cambridge, MA: Hogreffe and Huber Publishers. Callaghan, T. C., & Rochat, P. (2008). Children’s understanding of artist-picture relations: Implications for their theories of pictures. In C. Milbrath & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), Children’s understanding and production of pictures, drawings, and art (pp. 186–205). Cambridge, MA: Hogreffe and Huber Publishers.
go back to reference DeLoache, J. S. (2002). The symbol-mindedness of young children. In W. Hartup & R. A. Weinberg (Eds.), Child psychology in retrospect and prospect: In celebration of the 75th anniversary of the institute of child development (vol. 32). (pp. 73–101). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. DeLoache, J. S. (2002). The symbol-mindedness of young children. In W. Hartup & R. A. Weinberg (Eds.), Child psychology in retrospect and prospect: In celebration of the 75th anniversary of the institute of child development (vol. 32). (pp. 73–101). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). The British picture vocabulary scale (2nd ed.). Windsor: NFER-Nelson. Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). The British picture vocabulary scale (2nd ed.). Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
go back to reference Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Language and communication in autistic disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B, 346, 97–104. doi:10.1098/rstb. 1994.0133.CrossRef Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Language and communication in autistic disorders. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B, 346, 97–104. doi:10.​1098/​rstb. 1994.0133.CrossRef
go back to reference Frost, L., & Bondy, A. (2002). PECS training manual. Newark, DE: Pyramid Educational Consultants. Frost, L., & Bondy, A. (2002). PECS training manual. Newark, DE: Pyramid Educational Consultants.
go back to reference Fuller, D. R. (1997). Initial study into the effects of translucency and complexity on the learning of Blissymbols by children and adults with normal cognitive abilities. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 13, 30–39. doi:10.1080/07434619712331277818.CrossRef Fuller, D. R. (1997). Initial study into the effects of translucency and complexity on the learning of Blissymbols by children and adults with normal cognitive abilities. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 13, 30–39. doi:10.​1080/​0743461971233127​7818.CrossRef
go back to reference Ganea, P. A., Pickard, M. B., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Transfer between picture books and the real world by very young children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 180–185. doi:10.1080/15248370701836592. Ganea, P. A., Pickard, M. B., & DeLoache, J. S. (2008). Transfer between picture books and the real world by very young children. Journal of Cognition and Development, 8, 180–185. doi:10.​1080/​1524837070183659​2.
go back to reference Gelman, S. (2003). The essential child. Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York: Oxford University Press. Gelman, S. (2003). The essential child. Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Kanner, L. (1946). Irrelevant and metaphorical language in early infantile autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 103, 242–246.PubMedCrossRef Kanner, L. (1946). Irrelevant and metaphorical language in early infantile autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 103, 242–246.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., & Paul, R. (1997). Language and communication in autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (2nd ed., pp. 195–225). New York: Wiley. Lord, C., & Paul, R. (1997). Language and communication in autism. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (2nd ed., pp. 195–225). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview—revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685. doi:10.1007/BF02172145.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview—revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659–685. doi:10.​1007/​BF02172145.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2002). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services (WPS edition). Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (2002). Autism diagnostic observation schedule. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services (WPS edition).
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., & Pickles, A. (2004). Trajectory of language development in autistic spectrum disorders. In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 1–38). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Lord, C., Risi, S., & Pickles, A. (2004). Trajectory of language development in autistic spectrum disorders. In M. L. Rice & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Developmental language disorders: From phenotypes to etiologies (pp. 1–38). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
go back to reference Mirenda, P., & Locke, P. (1989). A comparison of symbol transparency in nonspeaking persons with intellectual disability. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 131–140.PubMedCrossRef Mirenda, P., & Locke, P. (1989). A comparison of symbol transparency in nonspeaking persons with intellectual disability. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 131–140.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mottron, L., Belleville, S., & Ménard, E. (1999). Local bias in autistic subjects as evidenced by graphic tasks: Perceptual hierarhization or working memory deficit? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 743–755. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00490.PubMedCrossRef Mottron, L., Belleville, S., & Ménard, E. (1999). Local bias in autistic subjects as evidenced by graphic tasks: Perceptual hierarhization or working memory deficit? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 743–755. doi:10.​1111/​1469-7610.​00490.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 719–730.PubMed O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 719–730.PubMed
go back to reference Plaisted, K. C., O’Riordan, M. A., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 777–783. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00375.PubMedCrossRef Plaisted, K. C., O’Riordan, M. A., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 777–783. doi:10.​1111/​1469-7610.​00375.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Preissler, M. A., & Carey, S. (2004). Do both pictures and words function as symbols for 18- and 24-month-old children? Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 185–212.CrossRef Preissler, M. A., & Carey, S. (2004). Do both pictures and words function as symbols for 18- and 24-month-old children? Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 185–212.CrossRef
go back to reference Rescorla, R., & Wagner, A. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. Black & W. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Rescorla, R., & Wagner, A. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. Black & W. Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64–99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
go back to reference Rosch, E. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 111–144). New York: Academic Press.CrossRef Rosch, E. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 111–144). New York: Academic Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Schopler, E., Reichler, R., DeVellis, R., & Daly, K. (1980). Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood autism rating scale (CARS). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10, 91–103. doi:10.1007/BF02408436.PubMedCrossRef Schopler, E., Reichler, R., DeVellis, R., & Daly, K. (1980). Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood autism rating scale (CARS). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10, 91–103. doi:10.​1007/​BF02408436.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sigel, I. E. (1978). The development of pictorial comprehension. In S. Bikkar, B. S. Randhawa, & W. E. Coffman (Eds.), Methods of family research: Biographies of research projects: Vol. 1. Normal families (pp. 87–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Sigel, I. E. (1978). The development of pictorial comprehension. In S. Bikkar, B. S. Randhawa, & W. E. Coffman (Eds.), Methods of family research: Biographies of research projects: Vol. 1. Normal families (pp. 87–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Smith, L. B. (2000). How to learn words: An associative crane. In R. Golinkoff & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), Breaking the word learning barrier (pp. 51–80). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Smith, L. B. (2000). How to learn words: An associative crane. In R. Golinkoff & K. Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), Breaking the word learning barrier (pp. 51–80). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H. (1992). Autistic children’s talk about psychological states: Deficits in the early acquisition of a theory of mind. Child Development, 63, 161–172. doi:10.2307/1130910.PubMedCrossRef Tager-Flusberg, H. (1992). Autistic children’s talk about psychological states: Deficits in the early acquisition of a theory of mind. Child Development, 63, 161–172. doi:10.​2307/​1130910.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Volkmar, F., Klin, A., & Cohen, D. (2000). Diagnosis and classification of autism and related conditions: Consensus and issues. In D. Cohen & F. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. Volkmar, F., Klin, A., & Cohen, D. (2000). Diagnosis and classification of autism and related conditions: Consensus and issues. In D. Cohen & F. Volkmar (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
Metagegevens
Titel
Symbolic Understanding of Pictures in Low-Functioning Children with Autism: The Effects of Iconicity and Naming
Auteurs
Calum Hartley
Melissa L. Allen
Publicatiedatum
01-01-2015
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 1/2015
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-2007-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2015

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 1/2015 Naar de uitgave