Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2/2013

01-02-2013 | Original Paper

Non-word Repetition Impairment in Autism and Specific Language Impairment: Evidence for Distinct Underlying Cognitive Causes

Auteurs: David Williams, Heather Payne, Chloë Marshall

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 2/2013

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Language-impaired individuals with autism perform poorly on tests such as non-word repetition that are sensitive clinical markers of specific language impairment (SLI). This has fuelled the theory that language impairment in autism represents a co-morbid SLI. However, the underlying cause of these deficits may be different in each disorder. In a novel task, we manipulated non-word stimuli in three ways known to influence the repetition accuracy of children with SLI. Participants with SLI were affected differently by these manipulations to children with autism. Children with autism performed similarly to language-matched typical children in terms of levels and patterns of performance, and types of error made, suggesting that the underlying cognitive cause of non-word repetition deficits is different in each disorder.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Voetnoten
1
Three participants with ASD scored under the ASD cut-off of 15 on the SCQ, with scores of 12, 13, and 4, respectively. Two of these participants (who scored 12 and 13) were taking part in another of our studies and, as a result, had data from another measure of ASD severity; the Developmental, Dimensional and Diagnostic Interview (3di; Skuse et al. 2004), a more detailed parent interview schedule that is based on the Autism Diagnostic Interview (Le Couteur et al. 2003). Each of these participants scored well above the ASD cut-off on the 3di. The final participant (who scored 4 on the SCQ) did not have any other parent report data available. However, this participant had been diagnosed by a UK-leading clinician in London. This, combined with our clinical impression of the child, make us entirely confident of his diagnosis, regardless of his score on the SCQ.
 
2
It may strike the reader as out of keeping with a diagnosis of specific language impairment that four participants in the SLI group had a NVIQ score outside of the normal range. However, a notable proportion of individuals who manifest SLI early in life (during the period in which most receive a diagnosis) show below average NVIQ later in life (e.g., Conti-Ramsden et al. 2001; Mawhood et al. 2000), with NVIQ dropping as much as 20 points across time (Botting 2005). Therefore, to obtain a representative sample of children who receive a diagnosis of SLI, we did not set as an inclusion criterion that NVIQ be in the normal range. We did, however, decide that in cases where NVIQ was below 80, non-verbal abilities must be substantially superior to verbal abilities, reflecting the fact that language problems are identified as the most clinically significant feature among all participants with SLI.
 
3
In fact, it is important to note that ADOS-G and ADI-R appear to have a surprisingly low specificity, which raises questions about their suitability for use in research studies. In the largest study of its kind, Risi, Lord, Gotham, Corsello, Chrysler et al. (2006) found that, if used in isolation, the specificity of each measure was less than 50 %, with the identification of around 29 % of non-spectrum children as having autism. If used together, specificity is improved, but in over 15 % of cases the instruments disagree on spectrum vs. non-spectrum diagnoses. These measures (particularly the ADI) have come under recent scrutiny, with some offering what we view as persuasive arguments that measures such as the SCQ and 3di are preferable to ADOS and ADI for research purposes (Bishop 2011, May 30).
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Alarcón, M., Abrahams, B. S., Stone, J. L., Duvall, J. A., Perederiy, J. V., Bomar, J. M., et al. (2008). Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 150–159. Alarcón, M., Abrahams, B. S., Stone, J. L., Duvall, J. A., Perederiy, J. V., Bomar, J. M., et al. (2008). Linkage, association, and gene-expression analyses identify CNTNAP2 as an autism-susceptibility gene. American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 150–159.
go back to reference American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th edition, text revised) (DSM-IV-TR). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRef American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th edition, text revised) (DSM-IV-TR). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.CrossRef
go back to reference Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition: A comparison of tests. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 970–983.PubMedCrossRef Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition: A comparison of tests. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 970–983.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Nonword repetition in specific language impairment: More than a phonological short-term memory deficit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 919–924.CrossRef Archibald, L. M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2007). Nonword repetition in specific language impairment: More than a phonological short-term memory deficit. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 919–924.CrossRef
go back to reference Arking, D. E., Cutler, D. J., Brune, C. W., Teslovich, T. M., West, K., Ikeda, M., et al. (2008). A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 160–164.PubMedCrossRef Arking, D. E., Cutler, D. J., Brune, C. W., Teslovich, T. M., West, K., Ikeda, M., et al. (2008). A common genetic variant in the neurexin superfamily member CNTNAP2 increases familial risk of autism. American Journal of Human Genetics, 82, 160–164.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Meldrum, D., et al. (2006). Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: The Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet, 368(9531), 210–215.PubMedCrossRef Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Meldrum, D., et al. (2006). Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: The Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet, 368(9531), 210–215.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ballarati, L., Recalcati, M. P., Bedeschi, M. F., Lalatta, F., Valtorta, C., Bellini, M., et al. (2009). Cytogenetic, FISH and array-CGH characterization of a complex chromosomal rearrangement carried by a mentally and language impaired patient. European Journal of Medical Genetics, 52, 218–223.PubMedCrossRef Ballarati, L., Recalcati, M. P., Bedeschi, M. F., Lalatta, F., Valtorta, C., Bellini, M., et al. (2009). Cytogenetic, FISH and array-CGH characterization of a complex chromosomal rearrangement carried by a mentally and language impaired patient. European Journal of Medical Genetics, 52, 218–223.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Barry, J. G., Yasin, I., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Heritable risk factors associated with language impairments. Genes Brain and Behavior, 6, 66–76.CrossRef Barry, J. G., Yasin, I., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2007). Heritable risk factors associated with language impairments. Genes Brain and Behavior, 6, 66–76.CrossRef
go back to reference Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). Comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder 1: Children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127–145.PubMedCrossRef Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1975). Comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder 1: Children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 127–145.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1977). Comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder 3: Discriminant function analysis. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7(4), 383–396.PubMedCrossRef Bartak, L., Rutter, M., & Cox, A. (1977). Comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder 3: Discriminant function analysis. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 7(4), 383–396.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). Overlaps between autism and language impairment: Phenomimicry or shared etiology? Behavior Genetics, 40, 618–629.PubMedCrossRef Bishop, D. V. M. (2010). Overlaps between autism and language impairment: Phenomimicry or shared etiology? Behavior Genetics, 40, 618–629.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Bishop, D. V. M., Maybery, M., Wong, D., Maley, A., Hill, W., & Hallmayer, J. (2004). Are phonological processing deficits part of the broad autism phenotype? American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 128B, 54–60.CrossRef Bishop, D. V. M., Maybery, M., Wong, D., Maley, A., Hill, W., & Hallmayer, J. (2004). Are phonological processing deficits part of the broad autism phenotype? American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 128B, 54–60.CrossRef
go back to reference Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 391–403.CrossRef Bishop, D. V. M., North, T., & Donlan, C. (1996). Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 37, 391–403.CrossRef
go back to reference Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130, 858–886.PubMedCrossRef Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130, 858–886.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Botting, N. (2005). Non-verbal cognitive development and language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 317–326. Botting, N. (2005). Non-verbal cognitive development and language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 317–326.
go back to reference Boucher, J., & Warrington, E. K. (1976). Memory deficits in early infantile autism: Some similarities to the amnesic syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 73–87.PubMedCrossRef Boucher, J., & Warrington, E. K. (1976). Memory deficits in early infantile autism: Some similarities to the amnesic syndrome. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 73–87.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Butcher, L. M., Kennedy, J. K. J., & Plomin, R. (2006). Generalist genes and cognitive neuroscience. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(14), 5–151. Butcher, L. M., Kennedy, J. K. J., & Plomin, R. (2006). Generalist genes and cognitive neuroscience. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(14), 5–151.
go back to reference Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Weismer, S. E. (2005). Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 1378–1396.CrossRef Catts, H. W., Adlof, S. M., Hogan, T. P., & Weismer, S. E. (2005). Are specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 48, 1378–1396.CrossRef
go back to reference Coady, J., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43, 1–40.CrossRef Coady, J., & Evans, J. L. (2008). Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairments (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 43, 1–40.CrossRef
go back to reference Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., Simkin, Z., & Knox, E. (2001). Follow-up of children attending infant language units: Outcomes at 11 years of age. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36, 207–219.CrossRef Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., Simkin, Z., & Knox, E. (2001). Follow-up of children attending infant language units: Outcomes at 11 years of age. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36, 207–219.CrossRef
go back to reference Conti-Ramsden, G., & Hesketh, A. (2003). Risk markers for SLI: A study of young language-learning children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 251–263.PubMedCrossRef Conti-Ramsden, G., & Hesketh, A. (2003). Risk markers for SLI: A study of young language-learning children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 38, 251–263.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Elia, J., Gai, X., Xie, H. M., Perin, J. C., Geiger, E., Glessner, J. T., et al. (2010). Rare structural variants found in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are preferentially associated with neurodevelopmental genes. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 637–646. Elia, J., Gai, X., Xie, H. M., Perin, J. C., Geiger, E., Glessner, J. T., et al. (2010). Rare structural variants found in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder are preferentially associated with neurodevelopmental genes. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 637–646.
go back to reference Gallon, N., Harris, J., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2007). Nonword repetition: An investigation of phonological complexity in children with Grammatical-SLI. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21, 435–455.PubMedCrossRef Gallon, N., Harris, J., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2007). Nonword repetition: An investigation of phonological complexity in children with Grammatical-SLI. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21, 435–455.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a casual connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360. Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a casual connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360.
go back to reference Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). The influences of number of syllables and wordlikeness on children’s repetition of nonwords. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 349–367.CrossRef Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). The influences of number of syllables and wordlikeness on children’s repetition of nonwords. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 349–367.CrossRef
go back to reference Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (2000). Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-Second Edition. London: Pearson Psychological Corporation. Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (2000). Goldman-Fristoe Test of Articulation-Second Edition. London: Pearson Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174.PubMedCrossRef Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). The autism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). The autism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Leonard, L. (2000). Specific language impairment across languages. In D. Bishop & L. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention, and outcome (pp. 115–129). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. Leonard, L. (2000). Specific language impairment across languages. In D. Bishop & L. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention, and outcome (pp. 115–129). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment and their first-degree relatives. Autism Research, 2, 22–38.PubMedCrossRef Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and reading abilities of children with autism spectrum disorders and specific language impairment and their first-degree relatives. Autism Research, 2, 22–38.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E., Leventhal, B., DiLavore, B., et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–224.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E., Leventhal, B., DiLavore, B., et al. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–224.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Marshall, C. R., Harris, J., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2003). The nature of phonological representations in children with Grammatical-Specific Language Impairment (G-SLI). In D. Hall, T. Markopoulos, A. Salamoura & S. Skoufaki (Eds.), Proceedings of the University of Cambridge first postgraduate conference in language research (Vol. 1, pp. 511–517). Marshall, C. R., Harris, J., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2003). The nature of phonological representations in children with Grammatical-Specific Language Impairment (G-SLI). In D. Hall, T. Markopoulos, A. Salamoura & S. Skoufaki (Eds.), Proceedings of the University of Cambridge first postgraduate conference in language research (Vol. 1, pp. 511–517).
go back to reference Marshall, C. R., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2009). Effects of word position and stress on onset cluster production: Evidence from typical development, specific language impairment, and dyslexia. Language, 85, 39–57.CrossRef Marshall, C. R., & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2009). Effects of word position and stress on onset cluster production: Evidence from typical development, specific language impairment, and dyslexia. Language, 85, 39–57.CrossRef
go back to reference Mawhood, L., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—A comparative follow-up in early adult life. I: Cognitive and language outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 547–559.PubMedCrossRef Mawhood, L., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—A comparative follow-up in early adult life. I: Cognitive and language outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 547–559.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Morton, J., & Frith, U. (1995). Causal modeling: A structural approach to developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, volume 1, theory and methods (pp. 357–390). New York: Wiley. Morton, J., & Frith, U. (1995). Causal modeling: A structural approach to developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, volume 1, theory and methods (pp. 357–390). New York: Wiley.
go back to reference Newbury, D. F., Paracchini, S., Scerri, T. S., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Richardson, A. J., et al. (2011). Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk variants in reading- and language-impaired subjects. Behavior Genetics, 41, 90–104.PubMedCrossRef Newbury, D. F., Paracchini, S., Scerri, T. S., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Richardson, A. J., et al. (2011). Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk variants in reading- and language-impaired subjects. Behavior Genetics, 41, 90–104.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Newbury, D. F., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Paracchini, S., Buckingham, L., Clark, A., et al. (2009). CMIP and ATP2C2 modulate phonological short-term memory in language impairment. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 85, 264–272.CrossRef Newbury, D. F., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Paracchini, S., Buckingham, L., Clark, A., et al. (2009). CMIP and ATP2C2 modulate phonological short-term memory in language impairment. The American Journal of Human Genetics, 85, 264–272.CrossRef
go back to reference Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. L. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850–863.PubMed Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., & Cleave, P. L. (1995). Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850–863.PubMed
go back to reference Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2011). Non-word repetition in adolescents with specific language impairment and autism plus language impairments: A qualitative analysis. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44, 23–36.PubMedCrossRef Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., & Simonoff, E. (2011). Non-word repetition in adolescents with specific language impairment and autism plus language impairments: A qualitative analysis. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44, 23–36.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Berument, S. K., Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Berument, S. K., Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
go back to reference Scerri, T. S., Morris, A. P., Buckingham, L. L., Newbury, D. F., Miller, L. L., Monaco, A. P., et al. (2011). DCDC2, KIAA0319 and CMIP are associated with reading-related traits. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 237–245. Scerri, T. S., Morris, A. P., Buckingham, L. L., Newbury, D. F., Miller, L. L., Monaco, A. P., et al. (2011). DCDC2, KIAA0319 and CMIP are associated with reading-related traits. Biological Psychiatry, 70, 237–245.
go back to reference Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2006). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals-fourth edition (UK). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Semel, E., Wiig, E., & Secord, W. (2006). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals-fourth edition (UK). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Skuse, D., Warrington, R., Bishop, D., Chowdhury, U., Lau, J., Mandy, W., et al. (2004). The developmental, dimensional and diagnostic interview (3di): A novel computerized assessment for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 548–558.PubMedCrossRef Skuse, D., Warrington, R., Bishop, D., Chowdhury, U., Lau, J., Mandy, W., et al. (2004). The developmental, dimensional and diagnostic interview (3di): A novel computerized assessment for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 548–558.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, C. (1991). Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, and Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 369–373.CrossRef Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, C. (1991). Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, and Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 369–373.CrossRef
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Strategies for conducting research on language in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 75–80.PubMedCrossRef Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Strategies for conducting research on language in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 75–80.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X. Y., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260. Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., Buckwalter, P., Zhang, X. Y., Smith, E., & O’Brien, M. (1997). Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 40, 1245–1260.
go back to reference van der Lely, H. K. J., & Howard, D. (1993). Specifically language impaired children: Linguistic impairment or short-term memory deficit? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 1193–1207. van der Lely, H. K. J., & Howard, D. (1993). Specifically language impaired children: Linguistic impairment or short-term memory deficit? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 1193–1207.
go back to reference Vernes, S. C., Newbury, D. F., Abrahams, B. S., Winchester, L., Nicod, J., Groszer, M., et al. (2008). A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. New England Journal of Medicine, 359, 2337–2345.PubMedCrossRef Vernes, S. C., Newbury, D. F., Abrahams, B. S., Winchester, L., Nicod, J., Groszer, M., et al. (2008). A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. New England Journal of Medicine, 359, 2337–2345.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wechsler, D. (2004). Wechsler intelligence scale for children—Fourth edition (UK). London: Pearson Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (2004). Wechsler intelligence scale for children—Fourth edition (UK). London: Pearson Psychological Corporation.
go back to reference Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Further defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific language impairment subtype? Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 319–336.PubMedCrossRef Whitehouse, A. J. O., Barry, J. G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Further defining the language impairment of autism: Is there a specific language impairment subtype? Journal of Communication Disorders, 41, 319–336.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Whitehouse, A. J. O., Bishop, D. V. M., Ang, Q. W., Pennel, C. E., & Fisher, S. E. (2011). CNTNAP2 variants affect early language development in the general population. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 10, 451–456.CrossRef Whitehouse, A. J. O., Bishop, D. V. M., Ang, Q. W., Pennel, C. E., & Fisher, S. E. (2011). CNTNAP2 variants affect early language development in the general population. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 10, 451–456.CrossRef
go back to reference Williams, D. M., Botting, N., & Boucher, J. (2008a). Language in autism and specific language impairment: Where are the links? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 944–963.PubMedCrossRef Williams, D. M., Botting, N., & Boucher, J. (2008a). Language in autism and specific language impairment: Where are the links? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 944–963.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Williams, D., Happe, F., & Jarrold, C. (2008b). Intact inner speech use in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from a short-term memory task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(1), 51–58.PubMedCrossRef Williams, D., Happe, F., & Jarrold, C. (2008b). Intact inner speech use in autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from a short-term memory task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(1), 51–58.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Williams, D. M., & Lind, S. E. (2013). Comorbidity and diagnosis of developmental disorders. In C. R. Marshall (Ed.), Current Issues in Developmental Disorders (pp. 19–45). London: Psychology Press. Williams, D. M., & Lind, S. E. (2013). Comorbidity and diagnosis of developmental disorders. In C. R. Marshall (Ed.), Current Issues in Developmental Disorders (pp. 19–45). London: Psychology Press.
go back to reference Williams, D. M., Bowler, D. M., & Jarrold, C. (2012). Inner speech is used to mediate short-term memory, but not planning, among intellectually high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 225–239. Williams, D. M., Bowler, D. M., & Jarrold, C. (2012). Inner speech is used to mediate short-term memory, but not planning, among intellectually high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 225–239.
Metagegevens
Titel
Non-word Repetition Impairment in Autism and Specific Language Impairment: Evidence for Distinct Underlying Cognitive Causes
Auteurs
David Williams
Heather Payne
Chloë Marshall
Publicatiedatum
01-02-2013
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 2/2013
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1579-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 2/2013

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2/2013 Naar de uitgave