Skip to main content
Log in

Brief Report: Representational Momentum for Dynamic Facial Expressions in Pervasive Developmental Disorder

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Individuals with pervasive developmental disorder (PDD) have difficulty with social communication via emotional facial expressions, but behavioral studies involving static images have reported inconsistent findings about emotion recognition. We investigated whether dynamic presentation of facial expression would enhance subjective perception of expressed emotion in 13 individuals with PDD and 13 typically developing controls. We presented dynamic and static emotional (fearful and happy) expressions. Participants were asked to match a changeable emotional face display with the last presented image. The results showed that both groups perceived the last image of dynamic facial expression to be more emotionally exaggerated than the static facial expression. This finding suggests that individuals with PDD have an intact perceptual mechanism for processing dynamic information in another individual’s face.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

References

  • Adolphs, R. (2002). Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12, 169–177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R., Sears, L., & Piven, J. (2001). Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 232–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: APA.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Azuma, H., Ueno, K., Fujita, K., Maekawa, H., Ishikuma, T., & Sano, H. (1998). Japanese Wechsler intelligence scale for children—III. Tokyo: Nihon-Bunka-Kagaku-sha.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bould, E., Morris, N., & Wink, B. (2008). Recognising subtle emotional expressions: The role of facial movements. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 1569–1587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castelli, F. (2005). Understanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in autism and normal development. Autism, 9, 428–449.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corden, B., Chilvers, R., & Skuse, D. (2008). Avoidance of emotionally arousing stimuli predicts social-perceptual impairment in Asperger’s syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 46, 137–147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corona, R., Dissanayake, C., Arbelle, S., Wellington, P., & Sigman, M. (1998). Is affect aversive to young children with autism? Behavioral and cardiac responses to experimenter distress. Child Development, 69, 1494–1502.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freyd, J. J., & Finke, R. A. (1984). Representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 126–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fujita, K., Maekawa, H., Dairoku, H., & Yamanaka, K. (2006). Japanese Wechsler adult intelligence scale—III. Tokyo: Nihon-Bunka-Kagaku-sha.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B. (2004). Autism, movement, and facial processing. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1719.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B., Deruelle, C., & Grynfeltt, S. (2001). Motion and emotion: A novel approach to the study of face processing by young autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 37–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, T. L. (1990). Cognitive representation of linear motion: Possible direction and gravity effects in judged displacement. Memory and Cognition, 18, 299–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubert, B., Wicker, B., Moore, D. G., Monfardini, E., Duverger, H., Da Fonseca, D., et al. (2007). Recognition of emotional and non-emotional biological motion in individual with Autistic Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1386–1392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, M. H., & Freyd, J. J. (1987). Explorations of representational momentum. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 369–401.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kodama, H., Shinagawa, F., & Motegi, M. (1982). Japanese Wechsler intelligence scale for children—revised. Tokyo: Nihon-Bunka-Kagaku-sha.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kourtzi, Z., & Kanwisher, N. (2000). Activation in human MT/MST by static images with implied motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 48–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1997). Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non-autistic retarded, and typically developing children and adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 401–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mukaida, S., Kamachi, M., Kato, T., Oda, M., Yoshikawa, S., & Akamatsu, S. (2000). Foolproof utilities for facial image manipulation [unpublished computer software]. Kyoto: ATR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., & McCarthy, G. (2005). Neural basis of eye gaze processing deficits in autism. Brain, 128, 1038–1048.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., McCarthy, G., & Labar, K. S. (2007). Perception of dynamic changes in facial affect and identity in autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 140–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sakamoto, Y., Ishiguro, M., & Kitagawa, G. (1986). Akaike information criterion statistics. Tokyo: KTK Scientific Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sato, W., & Yoshikawa, S. (2007). Enhanced experience of emotional arousal in response to dynamic facial expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 31, 119–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Senior, C., Barnes, J., Giampietro, V., Simmons, A., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M., et al. (2000). The functional neuroanatomy of implicit-motion perception or representational momentum. Current Biology, 10, 16–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shinagawa, F., Kobayashi, D., Fujita, K., & Maekawa, H. (1990). Japanese Wechsler adult intelligence scale—revised. Tokyo: Nihon-Bunka-Kagaku-sha.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tardif, C., Lainé, F., Rodriguez, M., & Gepner, B. (2007). Slowing down presentation of facial movements and vocal sounds enhances facial expression recognition and induces facial-vocal imitation in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1469–1484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yoshikawa, S., & Sato, W. (2008). Dynamic facial expressions of emotion induce representational momentum. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 25–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shota Uono.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Uono, S., Sato, W. & Toichi, M. Brief Report: Representational Momentum for Dynamic Facial Expressions in Pervasive Developmental Disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 40, 371–377 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0870-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-009-0870-9

Keywords

Navigation