Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 5/2008

01-05-2008 | Original Paper

Theory of Mind in Williams Syndrome Assessed Using a Nonverbal Task

Auteurs: Melanie A. Porter, Max Coltheart, Robyn Langdon

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 5/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

This study examined Theory of Mind in Williams syndrome (WS) and in normal chronological age-matched and mental age-matched control groups, using a picture sequencing task. This task assesses understanding of pretence, intention and false belief, while controlling for social-script knowledge and physical cause-and-effect reasoning. The task was selected because it is entirely non-verbal, so that the WS individuals could not rely on their good verbal skills when performing the task. Results indicated a specific deficit in understanding of false belief within the WS group. There was also evidence of heterogeneity in the WS group, with the false belief impairment restricted to only a particular subgroup of WS individuals identified originally by Porter, M., & Coltheart, M. (2005). Cognitive heterogeneity in Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(2), 275–306.
Voetnoten
1
According to Tager-Flusberg (1999), people with PWS are characterized by salient food-related characteristics including hyperphagia (overeating) and assiduous food seeking behaviors. PWS involves either a deletion on chromosome 15 derived from the father or maternal uniparental disomy of chromosome 15. Physical features are apparent including facial features, short stature, and small hands and feet. People with PWS typically show higher intelligence than that seen in other genetic disorders, with a mean IQ around 70. There is a hint of uneven academic performance within PWS with strengths in reading and a weakness in arithmetic. There is no universal PWS cognitive profile, though cognitively some authors suggest strengths in spatial-perceptual organization and visual processing and a weakness in short term memory.
 
2
The change in location task runs as follows: Participant (P) is shown two boxes. Experimenter 1 (E1) hides an object in Box A, Experimenter 2 (E2) leaves the room, E1 moves object to Box B and asks P three questions: “When E2 comes back where will E2 look for the object?”, “Where is the object really?”, and “Where does E2 think it is?”. The change in contents task runs as follows: P is shown a packet of M & Ms (or a local equivalent) and asked what is inside. Most Ps give the reply: “M & Ms.”. E1 then shows P that this packet does not actually contain M & Ms, but small pencils. P is then asked what another child, a classmate, who has not yet seen the contents, will reply when shown the packet. P’s task is (1) to differentiate between the mental representation she or he has of the actual contents of the packet (pencils) and the mental representation that the other child will have of its contents as a result of expectancies from world knowledge (M & Ms), and (2) to predict that the other child’s response will be based on her or his mental state (what she or he thinks it contains) and not on the actual contents known to P.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). From attention-goal psychology to belief-desire psychology: The development of a theory of mind, and its dysfunction. In D. J. Cohen (Ed.), Understanding other minds (pp. 59–83). New York: Oxford University Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). From attention-goal psychology to belief-desire psychology: The development of a theory of mind, and its dysfunction. In D. J. Cohen (Ed.), Understanding other minds (pp. 59–83). New York: Oxford University Press.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioral, and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113–125. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioral, and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113–125.
go back to reference Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Mills, D., Galaburda, A., & Korenberg, J. R. (1999). Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Trends in Neuroscience, 22(5), 197–207.CrossRef Bellugi, U., Lichtenberger, L., Mills, D., Galaburda, A., & Korenberg, J. R. (1999). Bridging cognition, the brain and molecular genetics: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Trends in Neuroscience, 22(5), 197–207.CrossRef
go back to reference Borg, I., Delhanty, J. D., & Baraitserer, M. (1995). Detection of hemizygosity at the elastin locus by FISH analysis as a diagnostic test in both classical and atypical cases of Williams syndrome. Journal of Medical Genetics, 32(9), 692–696.PubMedCrossRef Borg, I., Delhanty, J. D., & Baraitserer, M. (1995). Detection of hemizygosity at the elastin locus by FISH analysis as a diagnostic test in both classical and atypical cases of Williams syndrome. Journal of Medical Genetics, 32(9), 692–696.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Davies, M., Udwin, O., & Howlin, P. (1998). Adults with Williams syndrome. Preliminary study of social, emotional and behavioral difficulties. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 273–276.PubMed Davies, M., Udwin, O., & Howlin, P. (1998). Adults with Williams syndrome. Preliminary study of social, emotional and behavioral difficulties. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 273–276.PubMed
go back to reference Doyle, T. F., Bellugi, U., Korenberg, J. R., & Graham, J. (2004). “Everybody in the world is my friend”: Hypersociability in young children with Williams syndrome. American Journal on Medical Genetics, 124A, 263–273.CrossRef Doyle, T. F., Bellugi, U., Korenberg, J. R., & Graham, J. (2004). “Everybody in the world is my friend”: Hypersociability in young children with Williams syndrome. American Journal on Medical Genetics, 124A, 263–273.CrossRef
go back to reference Howlin, P., Davies, M., & Udwin, O. (1998). Cognitive functioning in adults with Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(2), 183–189.PubMedCrossRef Howlin, P., Davies, M., & Udwin, O. (1998). Cognitive functioning in adults with Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(2), 183–189.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jones, W., Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., Chiles, M., Reilly, J., Lincoln, A., & Adolphs, R. (2000). Hypersociability in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(3), 30–46.PubMedCrossRef Jones, W., Bellugi, U., Lai, Z., Chiles, M., Reilly, J., Lincoln, A., & Adolphs, R. (2000). Hypersociability in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(3), 30–46.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face processing, and theory of mind in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(2), 196–208.CrossRef Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face processing, and theory of mind in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(2), 196–208.CrossRef
go back to reference Langdon, R., Michie, P. T., Ward, P. B., McConaghy, N., Catts, S. V., & Coltheart, M. (1997). Defective self and/or other mentalising in Schizophrenia: A cognitive neuropsychological approach. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2(3), 167–193.CrossRef Langdon, R., Michie, P. T., Ward, P. B., McConaghy, N., Catts, S. V., & Coltheart, M. (1997). Defective self and/or other mentalising in Schizophrenia: A cognitive neuropsychological approach. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2(3), 167–193.CrossRef
go back to reference Laws, G., & Bishop, D.V. (2004). Pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: a comparison with Down’s syndrome and specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 39(1), 45–64.CrossRef Laws, G., & Bishop, D.V. (2004). Pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: a comparison with Down’s syndrome and specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 39(1), 45–64.CrossRef
go back to reference Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1990). Prospects for a cognitive neuropsychology of autism: Hobson’s choice. Psychological Review, 97, 122–131.PubMedCrossRef Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1990). Prospects for a cognitive neuropsychology of autism: Hobson’s choice. Psychological Review, 97, 122–131.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference McKusick, V. (1988). Mendelian inheritence in man: Catalogues of autosomal dominant, autosomal recessive and X-linked phenotypes. Baltimore: John Hopkins Press. McKusick, V. (1988). Mendelian inheritence in man: Catalogues of autosomal dominant, autosomal recessive and X-linked phenotypes. Baltimore: John Hopkins Press.
go back to reference Morris, P., & Sigman, M. (1988). Natural history of Williams syndrome: Physical characteristics. Journal of Pediatrics, 113, 318–326.PubMedCrossRef Morris, P., & Sigman, M. (1988). Natural history of Williams syndrome: Physical characteristics. Journal of Pediatrics, 113, 318–326.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge MA: MIT Press. Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
go back to reference Plesa-Skwerer, D., Verbalis, A., Schofield, C., Faja, S., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006). Social-perceptual abilities in adolescents and adults with Williams syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(2), 338–349.CrossRef Plesa-Skwerer, D., Verbalis, A., Schofield, C., Faja, S., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006). Social-perceptual abilities in adolescents and adults with Williams syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23(2), 338–349.CrossRef
go back to reference Porter, M. A. (2004). PhD Thesis: The cognitive neuropsychology of Williams syndrome. Sydney, Macquarie University. Porter, M. A. (2004). PhD Thesis: The cognitive neuropsychology of Williams syndrome. Sydney, Macquarie University.
go back to reference Porter, M., & Coltheart, M. (2005). Cognitive heterogeneity in Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(2), 275–306.PubMedCrossRef Porter, M., & Coltheart, M. (2005). Cognitive heterogeneity in Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(2), 275–306.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Porter, M. A., & Coltheart, M. (2006). Global and local processing in Williams syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome: Perception, attention and construction. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30(3), 771–790.PubMedCrossRef Porter, M. A., & Coltheart, M. (2006). Global and local processing in Williams syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome: Perception, attention and construction. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30(3), 771–790.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shaked, M., & Yirmiya, N. (2004). Matching procedures in autism research: Evidence from meta-analytic studies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 35–40.PubMedCrossRef Shaked, M., & Yirmiya, N. (2004). Matching procedures in autism research: Evidence from meta-analytic studies. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(1), 35–40.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Sullivan, K., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1999). Second-order belief attribution in Williams syndrome: Intact or impaired. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 104(6), 523–532.PubMedCrossRef Sullivan, K., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1999). Second-order belief attribution in Williams syndrome: Intact or impaired. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 104(6), 523–532.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H. (1999). Neurodevelopmental disorders. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT press. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1999). Neurodevelopmental disorders. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT press.
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H., Boshart, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Reading the windows to the soul: Evidence of domain specific sparing in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 631–639.PubMedCrossRef Tager-Flusberg, H., Boshart, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Reading the windows to the soul: Evidence of domain specific sparing in Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 631–639.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition, 76, 59–89.PubMedCrossRef Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition, 76, 59–89.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tager-Flusberg, H., Sullivan, K., & Boshart, J. (1997). Executive functions and performance on false belief tasks. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(4), 487–493.CrossRef Tager-Flusberg, H., Sullivan, K., & Boshart, J. (1997). Executive functions and performance on false belief tasks. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13(4), 487–493.CrossRef
go back to reference Udwin, O. (1990). A survey of adults with Williams syndrome and ideopathic infantile hypercalcaemia. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 32(2), 129–141.CrossRef Udwin, O. (1990). A survey of adults with Williams syndrome and ideopathic infantile hypercalcaemia. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 32(2), 129–141.CrossRef
go back to reference Ypsilanti, A., Grouios, G., Alevriadou, A., & Tsapkini, K. (2005). Expressive and receptive vocabulary in children with Williams and Down syndromes. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 49(5), 353–364.PubMedCrossRef Ypsilanti, A., Grouios, G., Alevriadou, A., & Tsapkini, K. (2005). Expressive and receptive vocabulary in children with Williams and Down syndromes. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 49(5), 353–364.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Williams, J., Barrat-Boyes, B., & Lowe, J. (1961). Supravalvar aortic stenosis. Circulation, 24, 1311–1381.PubMed Williams, J., Barrat-Boyes, B., & Lowe, J. (1961). Supravalvar aortic stenosis. Circulation, 24, 1311–1381.PubMed
go back to reference Woodcock R. W., & Johnson M. B. (Eds.) (1989, 1990). Woodcock–Johnson psycho-educational battery—revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing. Woodcock R. W., & Johnson M. B. (Eds.) (1989, 1990). Woodcock–Johnson psycho-educational battery—revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
go back to reference Woodcock, R. W., & Mather, N. (1989b,1990b). WJ-R tests of cognitive ability—standard and supplemental batteries: Examiner’s manual, Woodcock–Johnson psycho-educational battery—revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing. Woodcock, R. W., & Mather, N. (1989b,1990b). WJ-R tests of cognitive ability—standard and supplemental batteries: Examiner’s manual, Woodcock–Johnson psycho-educational battery—revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Metagegevens
Titel
Theory of Mind in Williams Syndrome Assessed Using a Nonverbal Task
Auteurs
Melanie A. Porter
Max Coltheart
Robyn Langdon
Publicatiedatum
01-05-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 5/2008
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0447-4

Andere artikelen Uitgave 5/2008

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 5/2008 Naar de uitgave