Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 8/2006

01-11-2006 | Original Paper

The Perception of Animacy in Young Children with Autism

Auteurs: M. D. Rutherford, Bruce F. Pennington, Sally J. Rogers

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | Uitgave 8/2006

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Visual perception may be a developmental prerequisite to some types of social understanding. The ability to perceive social information given visual motion appears to develop early. However, children with autism have profound deficits in social cognitive function and may fail to see social motion in the same way that typically developing children do. We tested the hypothesis that children with autism fail to discriminate animate motion, using a novel paradigm involving simple geometric figures. The subjects were 23 children with autism (c.a. 70.7 mos.), 18 children with other developmental disabilities (c.a. 68.2 mos.), and 18 typically developing children (c.a. 46.4 mos.). Children saw two circles moving on a screen and were rewarded for identifying the one that moved as if animate. A control condition required children to identify the heavier of two objects. Children with autism initially showed a deficit in categorizing objects as animate (though no deficit on the control task), but showed no deficit in this ability after they had reached criterion in the training phase. These results are discussed in terms of the social orienting theory of autism, and the possibility that animacy perception might be preserved in autism, even if it is not used automatically.
Literatuur
go back to reference Abell, F., Krams, M., Ashburner, J., Passingham, R., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R., Happe, F., Frith, C., Frith, U. (1999). The neuroanatomy of autism: A voxel-based whole brain analysis of structural scans. Neuroreport, 10(8), 1647–1651.PubMed Abell, F., Krams, M., Ashburner, J., Passingham, R., Friston, K., Frackowiak, R., Happe, F., Frith, C., Frith, U. (1999). The neuroanatomy of autism: A voxel-based whole brain analysis of structural scans. Neuroreport, 10(8), 1647–1651.PubMed
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Joint-attention deficits in autism: Towards a cognitive analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 185–189. Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Joint-attention deficits in autism: Towards a cognitive analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 185–189.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). The theory of mind deficit in autism: How specific is it? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 301–314. Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). The theory of mind deficit in autism: How specific is it? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 301–314.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen, S. (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 248–254.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Baron-Cohen, S., Allen, J., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Can autism be detected at 18 months? The needle, the haystack, and the CHAT. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 839–843.PubMedCrossRef Baron-Cohen, S., Allen, J., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Can autism be detected at 18 months? The needle, the haystack, and the CHAT. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 839–843.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Berry, D. S., & Springer, K. (1993). Structure, motion, and preschoolers’ perceptions of social causality. Ecological Psychology, 5, 273–283.CrossRef Berry, D. S., & Springer, K. (1993). Structure, motion, and preschoolers’ perceptions of social causality. Ecological Psychology, 5, 273–283.CrossRef
go back to reference Bertenthal, B. I. (1993). Infants’ perception of biomechanical motions: Intrinsic image and knowledge-based constraints. In Visual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 175–214). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Bertenthal, B. I. (1993). Infants’ perception of biomechanical motions: Intrinsic image and knowledge-based constraints. In Visual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 175–214). Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
go back to reference Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., Spetner, N. B., & Thomas, M. A. (1985). The development of infant sensitivity to biomechanical motions. Child Development, 56, 531–543.PubMedCrossRef Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., Spetner, N. B., & Thomas, M. A. (1985). The development of infant sensitivity to biomechanical motions. Child Development, 56, 531–543.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14, 151–157.PubMedCrossRef Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14, 151–157.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Buitelaar, J., van Engeland, H., de Kogel, K., de Vries, H., & van Hoof, J. (1991). Differences in the structure of social behaviour of autistic children and non-autistic retarded controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 995–1015.PubMedCrossRef Buitelaar, J., van Engeland, H., de Kogel, K., de Vries, H., & van Hoof, J. (1991). Differences in the structure of social behaviour of autistic children and non-autistic retarded controls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 995–1015.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.PubMedCrossRef Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biro, S., Koos, O., & Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of “pure reason” in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237–267.PubMedCrossRef Csibra, G., Gergely, G., Biro, S., Koos, O., & Brockbank, M. (1999). Goal attribution without agency cues: The perception of “pure reason” in infancy. Cognition, 72, 237–267.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cuccaro, M. L., Cope, H., Donnelly, S., Wolpert, C., Gabriels, R., Wright, H. H., Abramson, R. K., Koch, A. A., Gilbert, J., & Pericak-Vance, M. A. (2004). The repetitive behavior phenotype in autism: Age-related changes. American Journal Of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 1, 101. Cuccaro, M. L., Cope, H., Donnelly, S., Wolpert, C., Gabriels, R., Wright, H. H., Abramson, R. K., Koch, A. A., Gilbert, J., & Pericak-Vance, M. A. (2004). The repetitive behavior phenotype in autism: Age-related changes. American Journal Of Medical Genetics Part B-Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 1, 101.
go back to reference Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, J. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 353–356. Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, J. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 353–356.
go back to reference Dasser, V., Ulbaek, I., & Premack, D. (1989). The perception of intention. Science, 243, 365–367.PubMedCrossRef Dasser, V., Ulbaek, I., & Premack, D. (1989). The perception of intention. Science, 243, 365–367.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 479–485.PubMedCrossRef Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 479–485.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dittrich, W. H., & Lea, S. E. G. (1994). Visual perception of intentional motion. Perception, 23, 253–268.PubMed Dittrich, W. H., & Lea, S. E. G. (1994). Visual perception of intentional motion. Perception, 23, 253–268.PubMed
go back to reference Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193.PubMedCrossRef Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–193.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711–720.PubMedCrossRef Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711–720.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Happe, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Johansson, M., Gillberg, C., Dolan, R., Frackowiak, R., & Frith, C. (1996). ‘Theory of mind’ in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neuroreport: An International Journal For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 8, 197–201. Happe, F., Ehlers, S., Fletcher, P., Frith, U., Johansson, M., Gillberg, C., Dolan, R., Frackowiak, R., & Frith, C. (1996). ‘Theory of mind’ in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neuroreport: An International Journal For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 8, 197–201.
go back to reference Hashimoto, H. (1966). A phenomenal analysis of social perception. Journal of Child Development, 2, 1–26. Hashimoto, H. (1966). A phenomenal analysis of social perception. Journal of Child Development, 2, 1–26.
go back to reference Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.CrossRef Heider, F., & Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.CrossRef
go back to reference Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: The Social Attribution Task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41, 831–846. Klin, A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: The Social Attribution Task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41, 831–846.
go back to reference Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P.C., Pickles, A., Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P.C., Pickles, A., Rutter, M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview–revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRef Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview–revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. Oxford: Basic Books. Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. Oxford: Basic Books.
go back to reference Miles, J. H., Takahashi, N., & Mudrick, J. D. (2000). Repetitive behaviors differentiate autism subgroups. American Journal of Human Genetics, 67, 588. Miles, J. H., Takahashi, N., & Mudrick, J. D. (2000). Repetitive behaviors differentiate autism subgroups. American Journal of Human Genetics, 67, 588.
go back to reference Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, P. W. (1997). Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non-autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 401–423. Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, P. W. (1997). Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non-autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 401–423.
go back to reference Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949–971.CrossRef Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949–971.CrossRef
go back to reference Mullen, E. (1989). Mullen scales of early learning. Cranston, RI.: T.O.T.A.L. Child, Inc. Mullen, E. (1989). Mullen scales of early learning. Cranston, RI.: T.O.T.A.L. Child, Inc.
go back to reference Mundy, P., & Neal, R. A. (2001). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation: Autism (pp. 139–168). San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press. Mundy, P., & Neal, R. A. (2001). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a transactional social-orienting model of autism. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation: Autism (pp. 139–168). San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press.
go back to reference Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989). The theoretical implications of joint-attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173–183. Mundy, P., & Sigman, M. (1989). The theoretical implications of joint-attention deficits in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 173–183.
go back to reference Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.PubMedCrossRef Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 27, 657–669. Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 27, 657–669.
go back to reference Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.PubMedCrossRef Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ricks, D. M., & Wing, L. (1975). Language, communication, and the use of symbols in normal and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 5, 191–221.PubMedCrossRef Ricks, D. M., & Wing, L. (1975). Language, communication, and the use of symbols in normal and autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 5, 191–221.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rime, B. (1985). The perception of interpersonal emotions originated by patterns of movement. Motivation and Emotion, 9, 241–260.CrossRef Rime, B. (1985). The perception of interpersonal emotions originated by patterns of movement. Motivation and Emotion, 9, 241–260.CrossRef
go back to reference Rochat, P., Morgan, R., & Carpenter, M. (1997). Young infants’ sensitivity to movement information specifying social causality. Cognitive Development, 12, 441–465.CrossRef Rochat, P., Morgan, R., & Carpenter, M. (1997). Young infants’ sensitivity to movement information specifying social causality. Cognitive Development, 12, 441–465.CrossRef
go back to reference Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1988). Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 201–221.PubMed Rumsey, J. M., & Hamburger, S. D. (1988). Neuropsychological findings in high-functioning men with infantile autism. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 201–221.PubMed
go back to reference Rutherford, M. D., & Rogers, S. J. (2003). The cognitive underpinnings of pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 289–302.PubMedCrossRef Rutherford, M. D., & Rogers, S. J. (2003). The cognitive underpinnings of pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 289–302.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Rutherford, M. D., Young, G. S., Hepburn, S., & Rogers, S. (in press). A longitudinal study of pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Rutherford, M. D., Young, G. S., Hepburn, S., & Rogers, S. (in press). A longitudinal study of pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
go back to reference Scambler, D. J., Rogers, S. J., Rutherford, M. D., & Wehner, E. A. (in press). Emotional responsivity in children with autism and other developmental delays. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Scambler, D. J., Rogers, S. J., Rutherford, M. D., & Wehner, E. A. (in press). Emotional responsivity in children with autism and other developmental delays. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.
go back to reference Scholl, B. J., & Tremoulet, P. (2000). Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 299–309.PubMedCrossRef Scholl, B. J., & Tremoulet, P. (2000). Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 299–309.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tremoulet, P., & Feldman, J. (2000). Perception of animacy from the motion of a single object. Perception, 29, 943–951.PubMedCrossRef Tremoulet, P., & Feldman, J. (2000). Perception of animacy from the motion of a single object. Perception, 29, 943–951.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language comprehension in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20, 318–337.PubMedCrossRef Ungerer, J., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language comprehension in autistic children. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20, 318–337.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wheaton, K. J., Pipingas, A., Silberstein, R. B., & Puce, A. (2001). Human neural responses elicited to observing the actions of others. Visual Neuroscience, 18, 401–406.PubMedCrossRef Wheaton, K. J., Pipingas, A., Silberstein, R. B., & Puce, A. (2001). Human neural responses elicited to observing the actions of others. Visual Neuroscience, 18, 401–406.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The Perception of Animacy in Young Children with Autism
Auteurs
M. D. Rutherford
Bruce F. Pennington
Sally J. Rogers
Publicatiedatum
01-11-2006
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders / Uitgave 8/2006
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0136-8

Andere artikelen Uitgave 8/2006

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 8/2006 Naar de uitgave

Acknowledgement

Acknowledgements