Skip to main content
Log in

The Effect of Family Disruption on Children’s Personality Development: Evidence from British Longitudinal Data

  • Published:
De Economist Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This research documents the effects of different forms of family disruptions—measured by separation, divorce and death—on personality development of British children included in the 1970 British Cohort Study. There are statistically significant correlations between family disruptions prior to the age of 16 and personality development in early childhood. Parental divorce has the largest negative effect on a child’s personality development. Family disruptions have smaller effects on personality development when children are older and patterns differ by gender. The relationship between personality development and family disruption is partially driven by selection. Placebo regressions reveal significant correlations between family disruption and personality development before disruption. The omitted variable bias is mitigated by investigating mechanisms through which the selection operates.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Losing one of two natural parents in the household does not necessarily imply that the lost parent is absent all of the time. Next to changes as a result of death, separation or divorce of parents could mean that the role of the lost parent in the child’s life has changed. In all cases, the lost parents could have been replaced with a stepparent of other mother/father figure. These influences are not taken into consideration in this study because we lack information in our data.

  2. Roberts et al. (2003) summarize findings regarding the development of Big Five personality traits, and show that in childhood and adolescence, individuals become more conscientious and agreeable. In terms of emotional stability, individuals either do not change, or become more stable.

  3. Measured as average score on 7 family activities, each scaled 1 “rarely/never” 2 “sometimes” 3 “often” (Cronbach’s alpha 0.66). Items: “As a family, how often do you”: go for walks, go on outings, go on holidays, go shopping, go to restaurants, have meals together, have talks together.

References

  • Almlund, M., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. J., & Kautz, T. D. (2011). Personality psychology and economics. IZA Discussion Paper No. 5500.

  • Amato, P., & Keith, B. (1991). Parental divorce and the well-being of children: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 26–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amato, P. R. (2001). Children of divorce in the 1990s: An update of the Amato and Keith (1991) meta-analysis. Journal of Family Psychology, 15(3), 355–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersson, G. (2002). Children’s experience of family disruption and family formation: Evidence from 16 FFS countries. Demographic Research, 7, 343–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antecol, H., & Bedard, K. (2007). Does single parenthood increase the probability of teenage promiscuity, substance use, and crime? Journal of Population Economics, 20(1), 55–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beller, A. H., & Chung, S. S. (1992). Family structure and educational attainment of children: Effects of remarriage. Journal of Population Economics, 5(1), 39–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benson, H. (2010). Family breakdown in the UK: It’s not about divorce. Report, Bristol Community Family Trust.

  • Björklund, A., Ginther, D. K., & Sundström, M. (2007). Family structure and child outcomes in the USA and Sweden. Journal of Population Economics, 20(1), 183–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Björklund, A., & Sundström, M. (2006). Parental separation and children’s educational attainment: A siblings analysis on Swedish register data. Economica, 73(292), 605–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borghans, L., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. J., & Ter Weel, B. (2008). The economics and psychology of personality traits. Journal of Human Resources, 43(4), 972–1059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, S. H., Chen, Y.-C., & Liu, J.-T. (2009). The impact of unexpected maternal death on education: First evidence from three national administrative data links. American Economic Review, 99(2), 149–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb-Clark, D. A., & Ribar, D. C. (2012). Financial stress, family relationships, and australian youths’ transitions from home and school. Review of Economics of the Household, 10(4), 469–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb-Clark, D. A., & Schurer, S. (2012). The stability of big-five personality traits. Economics Letters, 115(1), 11–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobb-Clark, D. A., & Tan, M. (2011). Noncognitive skills, occupational attainment, and relative wages. Labour Economics, 18(1), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conti, G., Heckman, J. J., & Urzua, S. (2010). Early endowments, education, and health. Working Papers 2011–001, Human Capital and Economic Opportunity Working Group.

  • Corak, M. (2001). Death and divorce: The long-term consequences of parental loss on adolescents. Journal of Labor Economics, 19(3), 682–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunha, F., & Heckman, J. J. (2007). The technology of skill formation. American Economic Review, 97(2), 31–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunha, F., Heckman, J. J., Lochner, L., & Masterov, D. V. (2006). Interpreting the evidence on life cycle skill formation. In E. Hanushek & F. Welch (Eds.), Handbook of the economics of education (Vol. 1, pp. 697–812). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

  • Currie, J., & Stabile, M. (2006). Child mental health and human capital accumulation: The case of ADHD. Journal of Health Economics, 25(6), 1094–1118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ermisch, J., Francesconi, M., & Pevalin, D. J. (2004). Parental partnership and joblessness in childhood and their influence on young people’s outcomes. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series A (Statistics in Society), 167(1), 69–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ermisch, J. F., & Francesconi, M. (2001). Family structure and children’s achievements. Journal of Population Economics, 14(2), 249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feinstein, L. (2000). The relative economic importance of academic, psychological and behavioural attributes developed on childhood. Discussion paper 443, London, UK: Centre for Economic Performance, London School of Economics and Political Science.

  • Flouri, E., & Buchanan, A. (2004). Early father’s and mother’s involvement and child’s later educational outcomes. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 141–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francesconi, M., Jenkins, S. P., & Siedler, T. (2010). Childhood family structure and schooling outcomes: Evidence for Germany. Journal of Population Economics, 23(3), 1073–1103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fronstin, P., Greenberg, D. H., & Robins, P. K. (2001). Parental disruption and the labour market performance of children when they reach adulthood. Journal of Population Economics, 14(1), 137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gammage, P. (1975). Socialisation, schooling and locus of control. Ph.D. thesis, Bristol University.

  • Gennetian, L. (2005). One or two parents? Half or step siblings? The effect of family structure on young children’s achievement. Journal of Population Economics, 18(3), 415–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ginther, D. K., & Pollak, R. A. (2004). Family structure and children’s educational outcomes: Blended families, stylized facts, and descriptive regressions. Demography, 41(4), 671–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, E. D., Lavy, V., & Paserman, M. D. (2011). Sixty years after the magic carpet ride: The long-run effect of the early childhood environment on social and economic outcomes. Review of Economic Studies, 78(3), 938–973.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, E. D. & Simhon, A. (2011). Does quality time produce quality children? Evidence on the intergenerational transmission of human capital using parental deaths. CEPR Discussion Papers: 8258.

  • Knudsen, E. I., Heckman, J. J., Cameron, J. L., & Shonkoff, J. P. (2006). Economic, neurobiological, and behavioral perspectives on building America’s future workforce. Procedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(27), 10155–10162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, S., Ross, C., & Mirowsky, J. (1999). Establishing a sense of personal control in the transition to adulthood. Social Forces, 77, 1573–1599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCulloch, A., & Joshi, H. E. (2002). Child development and family resources: Evidence from the second generation of the 1958 British Birth Cohort. Journal of Population Economics, 15(2), 283–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McIntosh, J., & Munk, M. D. (2007). Scholastic ability vs family background in educational success: evidence from Danish sample survey data. Journal of Population Economics, 20(1), 101–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murasko, J. E. (2007). A lifecourse study on education and health: The relationship between childhood psychosocial resources and outcomes in adolescence and young adulthood. Social Science Research, 36, 1348–1370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Office for National Statistics (2010). Birth summary tables, England and Wales 2010: Table 1. Summary of key birth statistics, 1938–2010. http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/rel/vsob1/birth-summary-tables--england-and-wales/2010/birth-summary-tables--england-and-wales-2010--13-07-11.xls. Accessed 15 Dec 2014.

  • Prevoo, T. & Ter Weel, B. (2014). The effect of family disruption on children’s personality development: Evidence from British longitudinal data. CPB Discussion Paper 295.

  • Roberts, B., Robins, R., Trzesniewski, K., & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality trait development in adulthood, chapter 26. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126(1), 3–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006). Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132(1), 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robins, R., & Trzesniewski, K. (2005). Self-esteem development across the lifespan. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 158–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Tizard, J., & Whitmore, K. (1970). Education, health and behaviour. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Specht, J., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2011). Stability and change of personality across the life course: The impact of age and major life events on mean-level and rank-order stability of the Big Five. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 101(4), 862–882.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Von Stumm, S., Gale, C. R., Batty, G. D., & Deary, I. J. (2009). Childhood intelligence, locus of control and behaviour disturbance as determinants of intergenerational social mobility: British Cohort Study 1970. Intelligence, 37(4), 329–340.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bas ter Weel.

Additional information

We would like to thank the editor and two referees of this journal for helpful feedback. In addition, Lex Borghans, Nicole Bosch, and Suzanne Kok have provided insightful comments on an earlier draft of this paper.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (pdf 226 KB)

Appendix: Rutter, Self-Esteem, and Locus of Control

Appendix: Rutter, Self-Esteem, and Locus of Control

Three measures are used to capture personality traits: self-esteem, internal locus of control, and a Rutter index for behavioural problems. Self-esteem measures an individual’s sense of self-worth, internal locus of control measures a child’s perceived achievement control, and the Rutter index gives an indication of behavioural problems.

Self-esteem assesses respondents’ self-esteem with reference to teachers, peers and parents and consists of 12 items, while locus of control measures children’s perceived achievement control with a list of 15 items. Items are scaled 0 “yes” 1 “don’t know” 2 “no”, except the first self-esteem item “Parent like to hear about ideas” and the locus of control item “Believe in planning ahead”, which are reversely coded. Higher scores thus indicate higher self-esteem and higher internal locus of control. Table 7 shows the inter-item correlations or covariances for these sets of items, and the Cronbach’s alpha statistic for the scale formed from them, both for ages 10 and 16. The self-esteem and locus of control scores are constructed by summing the scores on the set of 12/15 items, and then standardizing the resulting score to have mean zero and standard deviation one.

Table 7 Inter-item correlations and covariances of self-esteem and locus of control items

At ages 5, 10, and 16, the respondents’ parents, usually the mother, are asked to indicate to what extent a certain description applies to the respondent. At ages 5 and 16, the items are scaled 0 “does not apply” 1 “applies somewhat” 2 “certainly applies”. The age-10 items are scaled 1 through 100, where 1 indicates “does not apply” and 100 indicates “certainly applies”. Higher scores indicate more severe behavioural problems. Table 8 shows the inter-item correlations or covariances for the 19 items, and the Cronbach’s alpha statistic for the scale formed from them. The three Rutter scores, at ages 5, 10, and 16, are constructed by summing the scores on the 19 mother-rated items, and then standardizing the resulting score to have mean zero and standard deviation one. The correlation coefficients between the resulting measures for self-esteem, locus of control, and behavioural problems (Rutter) are reported in Table 9.

Table 8 Inter-item correlations and covariances of Rutter items
Table 9 Correlation coefficients between personality measures

For each set of of items, we apply multiple imputation through chained equations to impute missing values, using the remaining items as predictors for the item of interest. By doing so, we increase the number of observations with valid personality measures. For each item i within each set of Rutter/self-esteem/locus of control items, we estimate the following model to predict missing values:

$$\begin{aligned} {\textit{Item}}_{i}=\alpha _{i}+\sum _{j \ne i} \beta _{j} {\textit{Item}}_{j} + \epsilon _{i} \end{aligned}$$

This process starts with the item with least amount of missing observations. Missing observations are then imputed, and the process continues to the item with next fewest missing observations. For the age-10 Rutter score we apply the above model using linear regression, since the items are scaled from 1 through 100. Imputed values below 1 are set to 1, and those above 100 are set to 100. For the age-5 and age-16 Rutter scores, and the age-10 and age-16 self-esteem and locus of control scores, we use multinomial logit regression to impute missing values.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Prevoo, T., ter Weel, B. The Effect of Family Disruption on Children’s Personality Development: Evidence from British Longitudinal Data. De Economist 163, 61–93 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10645-014-9248-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10645-014-9248-2

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation