Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research 6/2008

01-12-2008 | Original paper

Schematic Processing: A Comparison of Clinically Depressed, Dysphoric, and Nondepressed College Students

Auteurs: Lisa Halberstadt, Gerald J. Haeffel, Lyn Y. Abramson, Basabi R. Mukherji, Gerald I. Metalsky, Benjamin M. Dykman

Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research | Uitgave 6/2008

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Reformulations of Beck’s theory (e.g., Dykman et al. J Pers Social Psychol 56:431–445, 1989) propose that depressed and nondepressed people are equally likely to use schematic processing to interpret information. The few studies to test this hypothesis have had methodological shortcomings. Past studies have not included a clinically depressed sample and have failed to assess a full range of potential biases (negative, neutral, and positive). To address these limitations, a recognition-of-information task was administered to clinically depressed, dysphoric, and nondepressed college students. Clinically depressed participants were significantly more likely than nondepressed participants to generate negative interpretations of a self-relevant ambiguous story. Clinically depressed participants also were more likely than both dysphoric and nondepressed participants to refute positive interpretations of the story. However, consistent with reformulations of Beck’s theory, dysphoric participants and nondepressed participants also tended to “go beyond” the information given in the story. Indeed, all three participant groups were equally biased in their interpretations. The difference among the groups was in the direction (negative vs. positive) and the heterogeneity of the biases.
Voetnoten
1
Participants from the original data set were excluded from the current data analyses if they failed to meet criteria for any of the three study groups or if they scored below a predetermined cutoff score for objectively true or false items on the RIT: ≤1 for logically true items and ≥ 1 for logically false items. The logically true and logically false subscales on the RIT provide objective indices of participants’ performance on this measure, with scores below or above the respective cutoffs indicating very poor comprehension and/or memory for the experimental text. We took a conservative approach and eliminated these participants (N = 117) from the data analyses because it would be no surprise if participants who had little memory for the objective facts of the RIT story, and therefore little relevant current situational information available to them, relied on their schemas more so than did other participants. These 117 participants consisted of 8% of participants with BDI scores of 10 or above and 9% of those with BDI scores less than 10.
 
Literatuur
go back to reference Alloy, L. B., Albright, J. S., Abramson, L. Y., & Dykman, B. M. (1990). Depressive realism and nondepressive optimistic illusions: The role of the self. In R. E. Ingram (Ed.), Contemporary psychological approaches to depression: Treatment, research, and theory (pp. 71–86). New York: Plenum. Alloy, L. B., Albright, J. S., Abramson, L. Y., & Dykman, B. M. (1990). Depressive realism and nondepressive optimistic illusions: The role of the self. In R. E. Ingram (Ed.), Contemporary psychological approaches to depression: Treatment, research, and theory (pp. 71–86). New York: Plenum.
go back to reference Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row. Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.
go back to reference Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.
go back to reference Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100.CrossRef Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100.CrossRef
go back to reference Bergman, E. T., & Roediger, H. L. (1999). Can Bartlett’s repeated reproduction experiments be replicated? Memory & Cognition, 27, 937–947. Bergman, E. T., & Roediger, H. L. (1999). Can Bartlett’s repeated reproduction experiments be replicated? Memory & Cognition, 27, 937–947.
go back to reference Betts, G. H. (1909). The distribution and functions of mental imagery. New York: Columbia University. Betts, G. H. (1909). The distribution and functions of mental imagery. New York: Columbia University.
go back to reference Bumberry, W., Oliver, J. M., & McClure, J. N. (1978). Validation of the Beck Depression Inventory in a university population using psychiatric estimate as the criterion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 150–155.CrossRef Bumberry, W., Oliver, J. M., & McClure, J. N. (1978). Validation of the Beck Depression Inventory in a university population using psychiatric estimate as the criterion. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 150–155.CrossRef
go back to reference Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika, 16, 297–334.CrossRef Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika, 16, 297–334.CrossRef
go back to reference Dozois, D. J. A., & Dobson, K. S. (2001). Information processing and cognitive organization in unipolar depression: Specificity and comorbidity issues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 236–246.CrossRefPubMed Dozois, D. J. A., & Dobson, K. S. (2001). Information processing and cognitive organization in unipolar depression: Specificity and comorbidity issues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 236–246.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Dykman, B. M., Abramson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., & Hartlage, S. (1989). Processing of ambiguous feedback among depressed and nondepressed college students: Schematic biases and their implications for depressive realism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 431–445.CrossRefPubMed Dykman, B. M., Abramson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., & Hartlage, S. (1989). Processing of ambiguous feedback among depressed and nondepressed college students: Schematic biases and their implications for depressive realism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 431–445.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R. A. (1978), A diagnostic interview: The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 837–844.PubMed Endicott, J., & Spitzer, R. A. (1978), A diagnostic interview: The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 837–844.PubMed
go back to reference Flett, G. L., Vredenburg, K., & Krames, L. (1997). The continuity of depression in clinical and nonclinical samples. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 395–416.CrossRefPubMed Flett, G. L., Vredenburg, K., & Krames, L. (1997). The continuity of depression in clinical and nonclinical samples. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 395–416.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Haack, L. J., Metalsky, G. I., Dykman, B. M., & Abramson, L. Y. (1996). Use of current situational information and causal inference: Do dysphoric individuals make “unwarranted” causal inference? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20, 309–331.CrossRef Haack, L. J., Metalsky, G. I., Dykman, B. M., & Abramson, L. Y. (1996). Use of current situational information and causal inference: Do dysphoric individuals make “unwarranted” causal inference? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20, 309–331.CrossRef
go back to reference Haaga, D. A. F., & Beck, A. T. (1995). Perspectives on depressive realism: Implications for cognitive theory of depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 41–48.CrossRefPubMed Haaga, D. A. F., & Beck, A. T. (1995). Perspectives on depressive realism: Implications for cognitive theory of depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 41–48.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Halberstadt, L., Mukherji, B. R., & Abramson, L. Y. (2005). Cognitive styles among college students: Toward an integration of the cognitive theories of depression with cognitive psychology and descriptive psychiatry. Unpublished manuscript. Halberstadt, L., Mukherji, B. R., & Abramson, L. Y. (2005). Cognitive styles among college students: Toward an integration of the cognitive theories of depression with cognitive psychology and descriptive psychiatry. Unpublished manuscript.
go back to reference Harris, R. J., & Monaco, G. E. (1978). Psychology of pragmatic implication: Information processing between the lines. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 1–22.CrossRef Harris, R. J., & Monaco, G. E. (1978). Psychology of pragmatic implication: Information processing between the lines. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 107, 1–22.CrossRef
go back to reference Hollon, S. D., & Garber, J. (1988). Cognitive therapy: A social-cognitive perspective. In L. Y. Abramson (Ed.), Social cognition and clinical psychology: A synthesis (pp. 204–253). New York: Guilford. Hollon, S. D., & Garber, J. (1988). Cognitive therapy: A social-cognitive perspective. In L. Y. Abramson (Ed.), Social cognition and clinical psychology: A synthesis (pp. 204–253). New York: Guilford.
go back to reference Ingram, R. E., Fidaleo, R. A., Friedberg, R., Shenk, J. L., & Bernet, C. Z. (1995). Content and mode of information processing in major depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 281–293.CrossRef Ingram, R. E., Fidaleo, R. A., Friedberg, R., Shenk, J. L., & Bernet, C. Z. (1995). Content and mode of information processing in major depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 281–293.CrossRef
go back to reference Ingram, R. E., Partridge, S., Scott, W., & Bernet, C. Z. (1994). Schema specificity in subclinical syndrome depression: Distinctions between automatically versus effortfully encoded state and trait depressive information. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 195–325.CrossRef Ingram, R. E., Partridge, S., Scott, W., & Bernet, C. Z. (1994). Schema specificity in subclinical syndrome depression: Distinctions between automatically versus effortfully encoded state and trait depressive information. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 18, 195–325.CrossRef
go back to reference Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., & Beck, A. T. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding use of the Beck Depression Inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–299.CrossRef Kendall, P. C., Hollon, S. D., & Beck, A. T. (1987). Issues and recommendations regarding use of the Beck Depression Inventory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 289–299.CrossRef
go back to reference Lawson, C., & MacLeod, C. (1999). Depression and the interpretation of ambiguity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 463–474.CrossRefPubMed Lawson, C., & MacLeod, C. (1999). Depression and the interpretation of ambiguity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 463–474.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lawson, C., MacLeod, C., & Hammond, G. (2002). Interpretation revealed in the blink of an eye: Depressive bias in the resolution of ambiguity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 321–328.CrossRefPubMed Lawson, C., MacLeod, C., & Hammond, G. (2002). Interpretation revealed in the blink of an eye: Depressive bias in the resolution of ambiguity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 321–328.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lewinsohn, P. M., & Teri, L. (1982). Selection of depressed and nondepressed subjects on the basis of self-report data. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 590–591.CrossRefPubMed Lewinsohn, P. M., & Teri, L. (1982). Selection of depressed and nondepressed subjects on the basis of self-report data. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 590–591.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Matt, G. E., Vazquez, C., & Campbell, W. K. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 227–255.CrossRef Matt, G. E., Vazquez, C., & Campbell, W. K. (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 227–255.CrossRef
go back to reference Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
go back to reference Owens, J., Bower, G. H., & Black, J. B. (1979). The “soap opera” effect in story recall. Memory and Cognition, 7, 185–191. Owens, J., Bower, G. H., & Black, J. B. (1979). The “soap opera” effect in story recall. Memory and Cognition, 7, 185–191.
go back to reference Read, S. J., & Rosson, M. B. (1982). Rewriting history: The biasing effects of attitudes on memory. Social Cognition, 1, 240–255. Read, S. J., & Rosson, M. B. (1982). Rewriting history: The biasing effects of attitudes on memory. Social Cognition, 1, 240–255.
go back to reference Ruscio, J., & Ruscio, A. M. (2000). Informing the continuity controversy: A taxometric analysis of depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 473–487.CrossRefPubMed Ruscio, J., & Ruscio, A. M. (2000). Informing the continuity controversy: A taxometric analysis of depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 473–487.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Santor, D. A., & Coyne, J. C. (2001). Evaluating the continuity of symptomatology between depressed and nondepressed individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 216–225.CrossRefPubMed Santor, D. A., & Coyne, J. C. (2001). Evaluating the continuity of symptomatology between depressed and nondepressed individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 216–225.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Spitzer, R. L., Cohen, J., Fleiss, J. L., & Endicott, J. (1967). Quantification of agreement in psychiatric diagnosis: A new approach. Archives of General Psychiatry, 17, 83–87.PubMed Spitzer, R. L., Cohen, J., Fleiss, J. L., & Endicott, J. (1967). Quantification of agreement in psychiatric diagnosis: A new approach. Archives of General Psychiatry, 17, 83–87.PubMed
go back to reference Spitzer, R. L., & Endicott, J. (1978). Research Diagnostic Criteria: Rationale and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 773–782.PubMed Spitzer, R. L., & Endicott, J. (1978). Research Diagnostic Criteria: Rationale and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 35, 773–782.PubMed
go back to reference Van Den Broek, P. (1994). Comprehension and memory of narrative texts: Inferences and coherence. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego, CA, USA: Academic Press. Van Den Broek, P. (1994). Comprehension and memory of narrative texts: Inferences and coherence. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego, CA, USA: Academic Press.
go back to reference Wang, C. E., Brennen, T., & Holte, A. (2006). Automatic and effortful self-statements in depression. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 35, 117–124.CrossRefPubMed Wang, C. E., Brennen, T., & Holte, A. (2006). Automatic and effortful self-statements in depression. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 35, 117–124.CrossRefPubMed
Metagegevens
Titel
Schematic Processing: A Comparison of Clinically Depressed, Dysphoric, and Nondepressed College Students
Auteurs
Lisa Halberstadt
Gerald J. Haeffel
Lyn Y. Abramson
Basabi R. Mukherji
Gerald I. Metalsky
Benjamin M. Dykman
Publicatiedatum
01-12-2008
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Cognitive Therapy and Research / Uitgave 6/2008
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-007-9153-y

Andere artikelen Uitgave 6/2008

Cognitive Therapy and Research 6/2008 Naar de uitgave