Skip to main content
Top
Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development 1/2023

Open Access 02-09-2021 | Original Article

Biased Maintenance of Attention on Sad Faces in Clinically Depressed Youth: An Eye-Tracking Study

Auteurs: Christina Buhl, Anca Sfärlea, Johanna Loechner, Kornelija Starman-Wöhrle, Elske Salemink, Gerd Schulte-Körne, Belinda Platt

Gepubliceerd in: Child Psychiatry & Human Development | Uitgave 1/2023

Abstract

The role of negative attention biases (AB), central to cognitive models of adult depression, is yet unclear in youth depression. We investigated negative AB in depressed compared to healthy youth and tested whether AB are more pronounced in depressed than at-risk youth. Negative AB was assessed for sad and angry faces with an eye-tracking paradigm [Passive Viewing Task (PVT)] and a behavioural task [Visual Search Task (VST)], comparing three groups of 9–14-year-olds: youth with major depression (MD; n = 32), youth with depressed parents (high-risk; HR; n = 49) and youth with healthy parents (low-risk; LR; n = 42). The PVT revealed MD participants to maintain attention longer on sad faces compared to HR, but not LR participants. This AB correlated positively with depressive symptoms. The VST revealed no group differences. Our results provide preliminary evidence for a negative AB in maintenance of attention on disorder-specific emotional information in depressed compared to at-risk youth.
Opmerkingen

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s10578-021-01229-z.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Introduction

Major Depression (MD) is among the most common mental illnesses in children and adolescents with a prevalence of 17% up to the age of 18 years [1]. It not only has as major impact on development during childhood and adolescence but it is also associated with adverse outcomes in adulthood regarding education, social life and physical [2] as well as mental [3] health. Considerable neurobiological and cognitive development as well as high neural plasticity during childhood and adolescence may explain why youth are more vulnerable to psychopathology [4, 5]. With modest effects and high recurrence rates, however, current standard treatment does not suffice [6]. In order to refine and complement existing treatment, a more detailed understanding of mechanisms underlying development and maintenance of depression in youth is needed.

Negative Attention Biases (AB) in Depression

Cognitive models of depression consider cognitive biases a key element in development and maintenance of MD [7, 8]. Cognitive biases describe a shift in three aspects of information processing: attention to, interpretation of and memory for emotional information [911]. This shift promotes the filtering of incoming information according to the depressive schemas that initially activated the biases and in turn results in confirming these schemas, facilitating negative mood [8]. The shift in attention towards negative compared to positive or neutral information is referred to as negative attentional biases (AB). According to the model of vulnerability to depression by De Raedt and Koster [12], attentional processes are influenced by genetic and neuroregulatory factors [e.g. hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis functioning and impaired attentional networks] and are bi-directionally associated with emotion regulation (e.g. rumination). In this framework, negative AB would be essential in linking psychological and biological vulnerabilities for depression and its recurrence [12].
Importantly, attention is not a unitary process but comprises multiple consecutive components: orientation, maintenance and detachment of attention [13]. Studies on adult depression find reliable evidence for AB in the maintenance of attention on negative disorder-specific stimuli, while results regarding orientation of attention are less robust [11, 13]. Since major development occurs in cognitive and emotional functioning [14] and in attentional networks [15] during childhood and adolescence, results from adult studies cannot be directly transferred to youth. Cognitive patterns might not evolve into stable, trait-like “cognitive styles” until adulthood [16], therefore these might have less influence on cognitive vulnerability in youth than in adult depression. Alternately, brain maturation and hormonal changes associated with an enhanced emotional sensitivity [17] might make youth more susceptible to negative cues in ambiguous emotional information, resulting in more pronounced negative cognitive biases.

Negative Attention Biases (AB) in Youth Depression

There is a growing body of literature on AB in depressed youth, yet findings are heterogeneous [18]: while some studies found youth with MD to show increased attention towards negative disorder-specific (sad) information [19, 20], another study found them to avoid sad stimuli [21]. An association between negative AB towards threatening information (angry faces) and depressive symptoms in unselected youth was observed by one study [22], while no evidence for AB was discovered at all in a different study [23]. These differences could be due to moderating factors like genetic differences in e.g. hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis reactivity [24], or due to the different methods employed or different aspects of attention measured (e.g. allocation, maintenance with or without distraction) in different samples. As such, the direction of AB, i.e. whether depressed individuals prefer or avoid (i.e., show AB towards or away from) negative stimuli is unclear. Results are also unequivocal regarding specificity, i.e. whether AB apply to disorder-specific stimuli (e.g. sad faces) or threatening stimuli (e.g. angry faces) or negative information in general.

Measuring AB

To assess AB, reaction-time (RT) based behavioural measures are used regularly, with speed of response acting as indicator for AB towards or away from emotional stimuli. Investigating AB by measuring RT involves drawing indirect inferences about the focus of attention at one distinct point in time, neglecting that attention consists of different consecutive stages [13]. In addition, RT is influenced by non-attention-related processes like the speed of motor response, which may be generally delayed in MD [12]. The most frequently used paradigm, the Dot-Probe Task (DPT) [25], in which a probe appears in the location of either an emotional or a neutral stimulus, has been shown to have unacceptable psychometric properties [2628]. The emotional Visual Search Task (VST) [29], an alternative paradigm measuring RTs to identify a target emotional stimulus among distractors, shows better reliability [30, 31].
A more direct approach to assess attention is registering overt visual attention by tracking eye movements [13]. In eye-tracking (ET) studies, free viewing paradigms like the Passive Viewing Task (PVT) [21] are used most frequently. Participants are presented with various emotional stimuli to look at without further instruction. This method is not influenced by (non-ocular) motor response and, more importantly, provides data of visual attention over a longer period of time, enabling to distinguish between the various components of visual attention (unlike RT based measures). As Högström et al. [32] elaborate, the vigilance-avoidance-hypothesis in research of AB in anxiety states that differentiating categorically between visual attention towards (vigilance) or away from (avoidance) salient stimuli might not grasp the attentional process accurately. Instead, there is evidence from eye-tracking research that individuals with anxiety show initial vigilance to disorder-related stimuli, followed by avoidance (see e.g. [3234]). As both, initial orientation of attention as well as maintenance of attention can be assessed via the PVT, it allows to investigate if the vigilance-avoidance-hypothesis or similar more complex attentional mechanisms also apply to youth depression.
Additionally, psychometric properties of the PVT have been found acceptable in adults [27, 35] and in youth [36]. To our knowledge, only two studies have employed this paradigm to investigate bias in sustained attention in youth depression: one found AB away from sad faces in youth with MD [21], the other found AB towards sad faces in youth at risk for MD, moderated by genetical differences in HPA-axis reactivity [24].

The Current Study

The study had two main aims. The first aim was to investigate whether negative AB are present in youth with MD and to characterise AB by (1) direction (attention towards or avoidance of negative emotional stimuli) and (2) specificity to emotions and to particular aspects of attention. To investigate specificity to emotion, we assessed biases for disorder-specific (sad) as well as non-specific (angry) faces. To investigate different aspects of attention, we used multiple age-adapted experimental tasks: the DPT as the most commonly used AB task, the VST, an RT-based measure guiding participants’ attention towards distinct emotions and the PVT, an ET based measure in which individuals freely gaze at the emotional stimuli.
We compared 9–14-year-olds1 with MD to two groups of non-depressed children and adolescents varying in their risk for depression: children of parents with a history of depression, known to have an increased risk for MD themselves (high-risk; HR), see e.g. [39] and children of parents with no history of any mental disorder with a low risk for depressive disorders (low risk; LR). The comparison of those three groups enabled us to pursue the study’s second aim: to determine whether AB are more pronounced in currently depressed youth compared to youth at elevated risk for depression. HR youth have been found to show AB for negative information, although their direction and specificity has not been established yet with evidence for AB towards sad [40, 41] or angry [42] stimuli, as well as for AB away from sad stimuli [43]. Negative AB in children and adolescents at risk for depression would indicate that AB might act as a cognitive vulnerability contributing to the development of depression, as suggested by theoretical models, e.g. [7, 8]. Even more pronounced AB in currently depressed children and adolescents however would indicate that these biases might be exacerbated as a consequence of depressive symptomatology.
No study to date has directly compared AB in depressed, HR and LR youth, however, studies on interpretation [44] and memory [45] biases have found negative biases to be present in both depressed as well as high-risk youth compared to low-risk youth but to be more pronounced in currently depressed youth than in high-risk groups.
With respect to our first aim we expected to find more negative AB in currently depressed youth compared to healthy youth (both high- and low-risk), in accordance with theoretical models [8] and previous research [18]. Regarding specificity, we expected AB for sad faces (i.e. mood-congruent depression-specific stimuli) rather than angry faces (i.e. threatening anxiety-specific stimuli) in both the MD and HR groups. Due to differences in direction of AB in previous studies, we did not make predictions on direction of AB, i.e., whether MD participants would look preferably towards negative stimuli or avoid them. With respect to our second aim, we expected negative AB to be present in youth at high risk for depression compared to youth at low risk for depression (according to [4042, 46]) but to be more pronounced in the MD versus HR group, as found for interpretation and memory biases [44, 45].

Material and Methods

Data on AB presented in this paper were collected as add-on to a study on cognitive biases in the offspring of depressed versus non-depressed parents [37]. Data from tasks comparing AB in MD, HR and LR youth are presented here. Data from interpretation bias tasks [38, 44] and tasks comparing AB in children and parents of HR and LR groups [36] are presented elsewhere. To ensure quality of our tasks and results, we determined psychometric properties of each task and parameter and only report those with acceptable reliabilities (PVT, VST). Additional tasks and results can be found in the supplement.2

Participants

In total, 124 children and adolescents aged 9–14 years were included in data analysis.3 Participants formed three groups based on their current mental health state and risk of developing MD: n = 32 youth with MD (MD group), n = 49 mentally healthy youth at high familial risk for MD (HR group) and n = 43 mentally healthy youth with low risk for MD (LR group). Sample size was based on an a priori power analysis (α error probability = 0.05; power = 0.8; two-tailed) for the comparison of HR and LR groups (a smaller effect size was expected for this effect than for comparison with the MD group). Based on effect sizes (ES) from previous studies with mood induction procedures [40, 41], we expected an ES of at least d = 0.7, resulting in a required sample of n = 34 per group.
Participants with MD were recruited mainly at a University Hospital for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in Germany, some by a cooperating licensed psychotherapist and some responded to public adverts. Data of the LR and HR groups was largely (87%) derived from a study evaluating the transgenerational transmission of cognitive biases [36, 38] in which children and adolescents participated with one of their parents. N = 29 HR participants were recruited from a previous study on a preventive intervention for this particular risk group [48], n = 12 of them had taken part in the programme by the time they were tested in the present study. Others, as well as children in the LR group, were recruited by local and online advertisements and by letters to randomly selected families with children in the corresponding age range provided by the local registry office.
Youth were included in the MD group if they currently met criteria for MD according to DSM-IV [49]. Of those 32 youths, n = 4 fulfilled criteria for a recurrent course of MD, n = 2 were in partial remission, n = 15 had at least one comorbid anxiety disorder and n = 3 were treated pharmacologically. The HR group comprised youth who did not meet criteria for any axis I disorder, current or past,4 but at least one of their parents fulfilled diagnostic criteria for MD (n = 47; of which n = 8 fulfilled diagnostic criteria at the time of participation) or dysthymia (n = 2) during the child’s lifetime. Youth were not included if their parents had a history of bipolar disorder, schizophrenia or substance abuse. Participants were included in the LR group if they did not meet criteria for any axis I disorder currently or in the past and none of their parents met criteria for any axis I disorder at any time. All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision. Exclusion criteria for participants in all three groups were an intelligence quotient (IQ) < 85,5 assessed with the CFT-20-R [50], a pervasive developmental disorder, attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder or a history of schizophrenia or bipolar disorder.
All procedures were approved by the ethics committee of the Medical Faculty of the LMU Munich. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and parents after a comprehensive explanation of the experimental procedures. HR and LR youth who participated with their parents in the previous study on transgenerational transmission of cognitive biases received a reimbursement of €50 while MD participants who took part without their parents only in this study received €30.

Psychopathology Assessment

Before inclusion, all participants underwent extensive diagnostic assessment. The K-DIPS (“Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes- und Jugendalter”) [51], a standardised, semi-structured psychiatric diagnostic interview for assessment of psychiatric diagnoses in youth, was administered to participants and one of their parents. The K-DIPS is a well-established German diagnostic measure based on diagnostic criteria of axis I disorders according to DSM-IV [50] with good interrater-reliabilities [52]. Interviews were conducted and evaluated by trained interviewers. Interrater-reliability was determined for 18% of HR and LR participants and revealed an accordance rate of 100% for lifetime diagnosis of depression (pre-defined criterion). Interviews from the MD group were not re-rated, but most MD participants were receiving treatment for a clinical diagnosis of depression that was confirmed with the diagnostic interview. Psychiatric diagnoses of LR and HR participants’ parents were assessed with the adult version of the interview, the Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen (DIPS), [53], with good interrater-reliability [54]. Accordance rate for lifetime diagnosis of depression was 94% in our sample. In the HR group, the DIPS was administered only to the parent affected by depression; in the LR group the DIPS was administered at least to one parent and whenever possible to both parents.6 The German version of the Beck Depression Inventory-II [55], obtained from both parents for 83% of LR youth, was used to measure parents’ depressive symptoms. Scores differed significantly (t64.5 = 6.0, p < 0.001) between parents of HR (M = 9.9; SD = 8.5) and LR participants (M = 1.9; SD = 3.5).
For the assessment of the participants’ depressive symptoms the German version of the Children’s Depression Inventory “DIKJ” [56] was used and symptoms of anxiety were measured by the trait scale of the German version of the State Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children, STAIC [57]. For 123 of 124 included participants, a score for depressive symptoms could be determined and for 119 of 124 children an anxiety score was available. In our sample, reliability of both self-report measures was excellent (DIKJ: Cronbach’s α = 0.96; STAIC-T: Cronbach’s α = 0.92).

Stimuli

Coloured photographs of children’s faces displaying sad, angry, happy and neutral expressions on black background served as stimuli in both tasks [58]. Half of the models were male, half female in each task. The stimulus set comprised photographs of 16 models with sad, angry and happy expressions in the VST and 24 models with sad, angry, happy and neutral expressions in the PVT.

Visual Search Task (VST)

An emotional VST, adapted from [29] was used to assess AB in an RT-based task with explicit instructions to attend to specific emotions. The task was administered during ET, but due to poor reliability of the ET indices, ET results are reported in the supplement.
Each trial started with a drift correction (small white circle in the screen’s centre). Upon fixation of the circle, the trial was initiated. A fixation cross was then presented for 500 ms in the screen’s centre. Subsequently, the stimuli were presented in a 4 × 4 grid containing 15 distractors and one target (see Fig. 1). Each stimulus display contained all 16 models randomly allocated to the 16 grid positions. The participants’ task was to identify the face showing a certain emotion (i.e., the target) as quickly as possible and click on it with the mouse. Time to identify the target was unlimited. The experiment consisted of four blocks: Two blocks with happy targets and negative (either sad or angry) distractors and two with negative targets and happy distractors. Each block comprised 32 trials with the target appearing twice in each position and being twice each model. Blocks as well as trials within the blocks appeared in random order. Before each block, participants completed three practice trials.

Data Processing and Outcome Variables

Trials with incorrect responses or RTs shorter than 200 ms or longer than 2 SDs above each participant’s mean were excluded, in line with previous studies, e.g. [24]. Participants with poor accuracy (a correct and valid trial rate of 2 SDs below mean rate) were identified as outliers in terms of accuracy and excluded (n = 1 MD; n = 2 HR; n = 1 LR). In the remaining sample of 120 children, on average 119.6 correct and valid trials per participant (SD = 1.9; 93% of 128 trials; no group differences, Fs < 1, ps > 0.1) were available for analysis of the behavioural data.
A bias score (ABVST) was calculated by subtracting the mean RT in blocks with negative targets and positive distractors from the mean RT in blocks with positive targets and negative distractors [29, 59]. Hence, positive bias scores indicate more distraction by negative information (i.e. negative AB) and negative bias scores indicate more distraction by positive information (i.e. positive AB). ABVST scores for sad and angry faces were computed separately.

Reliability

To investigate internal consistency for the VST, split-half reliability was assessed by correlating bias scores based on odd versus even trials [60]. Reliability of the behavioural bias score was acceptable for both emotions (sad: r = 0.83, p < 0.01, Spearman-Brown corrected: 0.91; angry: r = 75, p < 0.01, Spearman–Brown corrected: 0.86).

Passive Viewing Task (PVT)

As a purely ET-based measure of AB, enabling participants to freely gaze at the emotional stimuli, a PVT was administered [21]. Each trial began with a drift correction (see above). A fixation cross followed for 1000 ms. Then the 2 × 2 stimulus array was presented for 15000 ms. The task consisted of 16 emotional trials, corresponding to the minimum trial number suggested for ET research [61] and eight neutral trials (not analysed), presented in random order. In the emotional trials, each array consisted of four photographs of the same model displaying a sad, angry, happy and neutral facial expression (see Fig. 2). The position of each emotional facial expression was randomly assigned to one of the quadrants with each emotion being presented four times in each quadrant. The neutral trials comprised four photographs of the same model with a neutral facial expression. Stimuli were sized approximately 9.5 cm × 7.5 cm and were presented with a distance of approximately 6.5 cm horizontally and 1 cm vertically between. Participants were instructed to fixate the white circle and fixation cross and then freely view the stimuli, required only to keep their attention focussed on the screen.

Eye-Tracker

Eye movements were registered with an EyeLink 1000 Plus Desktop mounted eye-tracker (SR Research). Participants were seated in front of a 15-inch monitor (1024 × 768 pixel resolution) on which the experiments were presented using Experiment Builder 1.10 (SR Research Ltd., 2013). Viewing was binocular while eye movements were registered from the dominant eye via infrared video-based tracking technology with a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. A forehead and chin rest minimized head movements and kept the viewing distance constant at 65 cm. Lighting of the room was kept constant for all participants. Before each task, a 9-point calibration and validation procedure was conducted and calibration was accepted if the average error was less than 0.5° of visual angle and the maximum error was less than 1° of visual angle.
Eye movement events were detected using a velocity and acceleration-based saccade detection method with saccades defined, in line with other studies [e.g., 28, 62], as events with a velocity above the threshold of 30°/s or an acceleration above the threshold of 8000°/s2. Gaze positions that were stable within 1° of visual angle for at least 100 ms were defined as fixations (in line with other studies [e.g., 63].
To ensure data quality, trials with a total dwell time of less than 75% of the presentation time were excluded due to excessive blinks or missing data [62] and ET data of each participant was inspected for systematic calibration errors. Subsequently, participants with less than 70% valid trials [64] and systematic calibration errors were excluded from the analyses of the eye-tracking data (n = 3 MD, n = 3 HR and n = 1 LR).7

Data Processing and Outcome Variables

After exclusion, a remaining sample of 115 children with on average 15.3 trials (SD = 1.1; 95.6% of 16 trials) per participant were available. This did not differ between groups (F ≤ 1.8; ps > 0.1).
As an indicator of initial orientation of attention, percentages of location of the first fixation on each emotion were assessed. As this parameter showed poor reliability, results are reported only in Supplement 2. As indicator of maintenance of attention, percentages of dwell time (defined as summation of all fixations) on each of the four emotions were assessed. For appraising the course of attention over time, trials were divided into five time windows (each 3000 ms).

Reliability

To assess reliability of the PVT, Cronbach’s alpha was calculated for percentage of dwell time on all four emotions (as for this task no AB scores were computed). Reliabilities for percentage of dwell time ranged from acceptable to good (angry: α = 0.69, happy: α = 0.86, neutral: α = 0.83, sad: α = 0.86).

Procedure

The course of the experimental session corresponds to [38]. Tasks were presented in random order and a mood induction procedure was administered twice during the experimental session. Participants watched a 2 min scene from the movie The Lion King [64] that had successfully induced unpleasant mood in children and adolescents before [65]. In our study, participants in both healthy groups reported a significantly worse mood, assessed using the valence dimension of the 9-point Self-Assessment Mannequin scale [66] after watching the movie scene compared to baseline (Fs ≤ 43.22; ps < 0.001). Mood did not change significantly, however, in the MD group (F2.4 = 1.15; p = 0.33). Details are presented in Supplement 5.

Data Analysis

Statistical data analysis was conducted with SPSS25. Significance level was set to p = 0.05 (two-tailed).
For the VST, one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted to assess group differences in ABVST scores. To assess relationships between psychopathology and AB, correlations were calculated of ABVST score with depression and anxiety scores.
For the PVT, a repeated-measures ANOVA with within-subject factors Time Window (5: time windows 1–5) and Emotion (4: angry, happy, neutral, sad) and the between-subjects factor Group (3: MD, HR, LR) were performed for percentages of dwell time. Degrees of freedom were adjusted via Greenhouse–Geisser correction when necessary. As this study’s main focus was a group comparison, only significant effects involving the factor Group were followed up using post-hoc ANOVAs and t-tests. To assess relationships with psychopathology, correlations of dwell time on each emotion (averaged across time windows) with depression and anxiety symptoms were computed.
All analyses were repeated excluding participants with psychopharmacological treatment (n = 3), as this has been found to reduce negative cognitive biases [67] and enhance attention toward positive stimuli [68]. The pattern of results in our sample remained the same, so findings are reported based on the whole sample. We did not include age as a covariate in our analyses as this is not appropriate when the between-groups factor (group) and covariate (age) are non-independent [69]. However, we performed additional analyses to investigate if age may have accounted for our results (see below).

Results

Sample Characteristics

Sample characteristics are presented in Table 1. Groups did not differ in gender ratio or IQ but in age: participants in the MD group were significantly older than participants of the HR and LR groups. As expected, the groups also differed in psychopathology: the MD group reported higher depression and anxiety symptoms than the healthy groups, which did not differ from each other, indicating that the HR group was as psychiatrically healthy as the LR group.
Table 1
Demographic and clinical characteristics
 
MD
HR
LR
ANOVA
Post hoc tests
 
n = 32
n = 49
n = 43
 
Gender m/f
6/26
19/30
18/25
χ2 = 4.9
n.s
 
Age; M (SD)
13.4 (1.4)
11.8 (1.7)
12.1 (1.7)
F2,121 = 9.8
p < .001
MD > HR = LR
IQ; M (SD)
105.2 (13.6)
108.8 (11.5)
111.8 (10.2)
F2,121 = 2.9
n.s
 
Depressive
symptoms; M (SD)
31.5 (8.9)
7.9 (5.7)
6.6 (5.2)
F2,120 = 163.5
p < .001
MD > HR = LR
Anxiety; M (SD)
45.1 (8.8)
30.0 (6.4)
28.1 (6.2)
F2,116 = 56.6
p < .001
MD > HR = LR
Depressive symptoms were assessed with DIKJ (raw values presented), anxiety with STAIC-T. All post-hoc tests were significant (p < .001)
MD major depression group, HR high-risk group, LR low-risk group, M Mean, SD Standard Deviation

Visual Search Task

One-way ANOVAs on ABVST scores revealed no significant differences between MD, HR and LR youth (Fs < 1; ps > 0.1) regarding sad or angry faces. The ABVST for sad faces was significantly < 0 in the LR group (t41 = 2.5, p = 0.016), indicating a positive AB for sad faces (i.e. away from sad faces/towards happy faces), but bias scores for sad faces in the other groups as well as for angry faces were not significantly different from 0 (ts ≤ 1.6, ps > 0.1). Bias scores are presented in Table 2. The correlational analyses revealed no relationship between ABVST scores for sad or angry faces with depressive or anxiety symptoms (r ≤ 0.17; p ≥ 0.072).8
Table 2
AB scores from the Visual Search Task
ABVST
MD
HR
LR
 
n = 31
n = 47
n = 42
ABVST for sad faces M (SD)
 − 47.0 (1040.1)
 − 259.2 (1098.6)
 − 323.8 (832.6)
ABVST for angry faces M (SD)
 − 11.0 (882.2)
227.4 (1227.9)
89.2 (884.6)
ABVST attention bias score from the Visual Search Task, MD major depression group, HR high-risk group, LR low-risk group, M Mean, SD Standard Deviation

Passive Viewing Task

The TimeWindow × Emotion × Group ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of TimeWindow (F1.9,207.3 = 24.3, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.18) and Emotion (F2.6,295.1 = 4.5, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.04). Significant TimeWindow × Emotion (F9.6,1076.0 = 1.9, p = 0.040, ηp2 = 0.02) and Emotion × Group (F5.3,295.1 = 2.5, p = 0.027, ηp2 = 0.04) interactions emerged. All other main effects and interactions were non-significant (Fs ≤ 2.3; ps ≥ 0.1). To follow up the Emotion × Group interaction, ANOVAs with the factor Group were calculated separately for each emotion on percentage of dwell time averaged across time windows: a significant effect emerged of Group for sad faces (F2,112 = 4.7, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.08), but no other emotion (Fs ≤ 2.5, ps ≥ 0.08). Post-hoc t-tests revealed a significant difference in percentage of dwell time on sad faces between the MD and HR groups (t36.2 = 2.3, p = 0.027, d = 0.6) with the MD group dwelling more on sad faces than the HR group. Differences between MD vs. LR groups (t33.4 = 1.5, p = 0.145, d = 0.4) and between HR and LR groups (t85 = 1.7, p = 0.090, d = 0.4) were non-significant, with a trend for HR participants dwelling less on sad faces than LR participants. Percentages of dwell time are presented in Fig. 3. Furthermore, positive correlations of dwell time on sad faces with depressive (r = 0.29, p = 0.002) and anxiety symptoms (r = 0.38, p < 0.001) and a small negative correlation between dwell time on happy faces and depressive symptoms (r = -0.21 p = 0.023) were found among all participants.9

Additional Analyses

As groups differed in age, we performed additional analyses to investigate if this may have accounted for the group differences in dwell times on sad faces. Pearson’s correlations between age and dwell time on sad faces were computed separately within each group (to rule out that correlations were artifacts of group differences): no significant correlations emerged (rs ≤ 0.23, ps > 0.1).

Discussion

The overarching goal of the present study was to investigate the nature of negative AB in depressed youth. With two different experimental approaches, a passive viewing ET paradigm and an RT-based task externally guiding attention, we evaluated AB in three groups of 9–14-year-olds: currently depressed youth (MD group), mentally healthy youth with a high familial risk for depression (HR group) and with a low familial risk for depression (LR group). While the behavioural paradigm revealed no evidence of AB in any group, the eye-tracking paradigm revealed a more negative bias towards disorder-specific stimuli in depressed compared to high-risk youth in maintenance of attention. This measure correlated with depressive symptoms across groups.
Our first aim was to characterise AB in depressed youth regarding presence, direction, specificity to disorder-related information and specificity to particular attentional components. Our second aim was to determine to which extent AB are more pronounced in currently depressed youth compared to at-risk youth.
We found a significant group difference in dwell time on sad faces in the PVT between youth with MD and at-risk youth: as expected, youth with MD dwelled longer on sad faces compared to youth at risk for depression. Contrary to our expectations, the LR group’s dwell time on sad faces was in between: they dwelled less on sad faces than the MD group but more than HR youth (although these differences were non-significant). No differences were found for the other emotions. These results partially support our hypotheses: they suggest that youth with MD might show more negative AB in maintenance of attention on disorder-specific negative stimuli in comparison to healthy children. However, according to our results, this would appear to be the case only for healthy children with a familial risk of depression.
Youth with MD preferentially attending to disorder-specific negative information is in line with several studies [19, 20], nevertheless, the only study that used a similar passive viewing ET paradigm [21] found that youth with MD looked less at sad faces, i.e. avoided them. These contradicting results might be explained by the age of the investigated samples: while the mean age of the depressed groups in the studies by Hankin et al. [19], Sylvester et al. [20] and ours was above 13 years, Harrison and Gibb [21] examined a considerably younger sample with a mean age of 11.2 years which might have captured youth before a “maladaptive shift” in their emotion regulation starting approximately from age 12 [70, 71]. Harrison and Gibb [21] argue that children may use avoidance of sad faces to regulate negative emotion and that this ability may diminish with age. Thus, it would be lost in older adolescents and adults with depression, leaving them to preferentially attend to negative information. A similar idea of a change in attention bias with age is found in research on AB for threatening stimuli in youth with anxiety disorders: as Dudeney et al. [33] showed in their meta-analysis, differences in AB to threat between children with anxiety diagnosis and control groups are found to increase with age. This change might be due to a general tendency in children to be more immediately reactive to external stimuli, which would normally decrease over time as their ability to inhibit attention and information processing develops. Children and adolescents who develop an anxiety disorder might instead maintain this heightened attention towards threat [34]. If this reactivity would not only apply to threatening stimuli, but also to other salient stimuli, the same development might potentially occur regarding AB towards sad stimuli.
Although not statistically significant, our HR group showed a slightly less pronounced negative AB compared to the LR group. This pattern of results was somewhat surprising as AB in high-risk youth are proposed to act as a risk factor for depression [46] and to be exacerbated during an episode of depression (as memory and interpretation biases [44, 45]) and are therefore expected to have the same direction as AB in currently depressed youth. As attentional deployment is an effective emotion regulation strategy (see e.g. [9, 72]), AB away from negative information might act as a protective mechanism against development of depression. Our sample of children of depressed parents tended to show AB away from sad faces, as described for mentally healthy youth [19, 73]. It is conceivable, albeit rather speculative, that they might have acquired this mechanism as a protective response to their parents’ disorder. This might add to their resilience, helping to explain why they are still psychiatrically healthy despite their heightened risk for MD.10 Maybe at-risk youth who do not apply this protective mechanism would be more vulnerable to depression and may have already developed a depressive disorder [74]. In line with this assumption, participants in our MD group did not show this protective mechanism—either because their ability to avoid negative information was already less pronounced before, adding to their vulnerability, or because their ability to regulate their emotions this way diminished as a consequence of depressive symptomatology [75]. Due to the cross-sectional design of our study, we cannot make conclusions about time course or causality, i.e. whether the more negative AB in the MD group compared to the HR group is a consequence of depression, present before disorder onset, or if the use of avoidance of negative stimuli as an emotion regulation strategy decreases with age as suggested by Harrison and Gibb [21]. However, these deliberations are highly speculative since differences between HR vs. LR and MD vs. LR did not reach significance, the difference between HR and MD groups would not have been significant if a correction for multiple testing had been applied and effect sizes for differences between groups were smaller than expected.
Other indices of attention we investigated yielded either non-interpretable results due to poor reliability (e.g., location of the first fixation in the PVT as index of initial attention allocation; see Supplement 2) or did not reveal any group differences despite acceptable reliability (e.g., the behavioural ABVST parameter). In the VST, the LR group was found to show a small positive AB regarding sad faces, but no significant biases emerged for HR or MD groups as well as no group differences. Since the VST was originally designed to modify AB and measure within-subject differences in a Cognitive Bias Modification Training [29, 76], it might not be sensitive to between-group differences. Moreover, the VST is based on distinct instructions which emotion to attend to, i.e. attention is guided externally, whereas in the PVT, participants can deliberately choose where to turn their attention. In our study, evidence for AB in depressed youth was found only with the PVT, not the VST, suggesting that depressed and high-risk youth differ only in an internally controlled and at least to some extent conscious aspect of attention. This is in line with Teachman et al. [77] who found evidence for a more conscious, intentional bias in attention. Importantly, the measure revealing group differences—percentage of dwell time on sad faces in the PVT—not only showed good reliability but was also related to depressive symptoms across groups, suggesting it is a valid indicator of “depressive” processing.11 Of note, dwell time as a measure of AB cannot be easily attributed a certain component of attention. Longer dwell times, i.e., longer maintenance of attention on certain stimuli, may result from participants more frequently looking at them, having difficulties disengaging their attention from them, or deliberately maintaining their attention on them. Future studies may try to tease apart those mechanisms in order to identify which is or are responsible for our results.

Strengths

To our knowledge, this is the first study directly comparing AB in currently depressed, high-and low-risk youth. It contributes to a more detailed understanding of AB in youth depression and their role in development and maintenance of MD. Another strength is our use of a standardised, semi-structured diagnostic interview with all participants, conducted by trained personnel to ensure diagnostic accuracy. Moreover, parental psychopathology was also assessed via a psychiatric interview in the HR and LR groups rather than relying on self-report measures. In addition, we used multiple measures of AB, assessing attention in deliberate vs. externally guided circumstances. We evaluated reliabilities of our measures, a practice often omitted in AB research [36], and only interpreted the results of those measures with at least acceptable reliability. This led to differing results, contributing to a more nuanced understanding of AB in youth depression.

Limitations

With most MD participants recruited at a Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry or through outpatient psychotherapists, most had received psychotherapy at the time they participated. Unfortunately, we did not systematically assess treatment type or duration. As psychotherapy is expected to reduce AB (e.g. in anxiety disorders; see e.g. [7880]) effect sizes might be underestimated in our study. Furthermore, we recruited the HR group partly from a study evaluating a prevention program for at-risk youth [49] some had already participated in. This Cognitive Behavioural Therapy based group intervention conveyed distraction as a coping strategy, among others, which might have influenced participants’ volitional attention maintenance and added to our HR group’s resilience, rendering them not entirely representative for children of depressed parents. Another limitation results from comorbid anxiety disorders in nearly half of the participants in the MD group, known to be associated with AB, as well [80]. However, anxious youth are found to show AB for threatening stimuli [8082] while depressed individuals are commonly found to show AB for sad stimuli [13]. Since we found AB for sad faces only, it is more likely that the AB we captured were related to participants’ depression rather than the comorbid anxiety disorders. In addition, our three groups differed significantly in age with the MD group being the oldest. As the incidence of MD increases with puberty [83] this does not come as a surprise. However, since the detected AB did not correlate with age, it is unlikely that this difference accounted for our results. Furthermore, it has to be emphasized that although our sample size is larger than previous studies’ (e.g., [21]: n = 19 in the MD group; [23]: n = 19 in the MD group; [73]: n = 10 in the high-risk group), the sample size was only powered to detect medium ES and may have prevented us from finding smaller effects. At the same time, it is possible that the significant effect we found is not a real group difference but a false positive result since we did not correct for multiple testing (in line with, e.g. [21]). Replication in larger samples of children and adolescents is therefore crucial to rule out that our result is a chance finding and to be able to draw firmer conclusions.
Finally, even though we determined and reported reliabilities of our measures and the correlation of the PVT maintenance score with depressive symptoms indicates that it could be a valid indicator of depressive processing, we cannot provide a full description of the psychometric properties of our tasks. The appropriateness of measures that have often been used to assess AB in the past, like the DPT, has repeatedly been called into question [2628], while at the same time no alternative measure has yet become the widely accepted norm. Therefore, future studies should further examine reliability and validity of tasks assessing ABs, refine existing tasks and develop new ones that can measure AB with less error. As this should reduce the variability of results, it would truly move the field forward.

Summary

In summary, our study provides preliminary evidence for a more negative AB in depressed youth compared to healthy youth at risk for depression. Our sample of youth with MD preferentially attended to sad faces, i.e., their AB was directed towards negative information. This AB was found only regarding sad, not angry faces, suggesting AB in youth depression may be specific to disorder-related negative emotional information. In addition, the more negative AB in depressed youth was found only for maintenance of attention in a task where attention was allocated deliberately, suggesting this AB might be specific to an internally controlled and at least to some extent conscious aspect of attention. Our results suggest that depressed and at-risk youth might show AB in opposite directions with AB towards negative information in depressed youth and AB away from negative information in at-risk youth, potentially acting as a protective mechanism in this group. However, this interpretation remains highly speculative and replication in larger samples is essential. Longitudinal research is necessary to address the temporal course of AB in youth, i.e. if youth with AB away from negative information are less likely to develop a depressive disorder than those with negative AB towards or if negative AB arises with depressive symptomatology. This will help shed more light on the role negative AB play in the development and maintenance of depressive symptoms. We think that despite the speculative nature of our interpretations they provide an interesting incentive for the elaboration of theoretical frameworks and starting points for future investigations.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all participants for their support! Furthermore, we thank Petra Wagenbüchler, Sonja Stolp and team for their help with participant recruitment and Petra Wagenbüchler, Veronika Jäger, Lisa Ordenewitz, Jakob Neumüller, Laura Asperud Thomsen, Moritz Dannert and Ann-Sophie Störmann for their help with data collection.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Onze productaanbevelingen

BSL Psychologie Totaal

Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.

BSL Academy Accare GGZ collective

Bijlagen

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Voetnoten
1
As the present study was planned as add-on to a study on transgenerational transmission of cognitive biases 37, 38, it focused on the same age range. Children younger than 9 years were not included due to concerns about their ability to understand and perform the tasks. Adolescents above 14 years were not included since the incidence of MD in children of depressed parents increases substantially after that age, e.g. 39, and investigating older mentally healthy children of depressed parents might result in examining a particularly resilient, thus non-representative sample.
 
2
In addition to PVT, VST and DPT, we aimed to assess AB with a computerized version of the Scrambled Sentences task (SST, 47) via ET in addition to interpretation bias (IB; see 44 for results on IB). Due to poor reliability, DPT and SST are included in the supplement.
 
3
126 children and adolescents were tested; two were excluded due to poor compliance.
 
4
One HR participant had met criteria for enuresis in the past, but did not report any other symptoms of any other mental disorder, so she was included nonetheless.
 
5
Two MD group participants scored just below 85. The substandard IQ did not correspond to the clinical impression and was most likely due to a lack of compliance and inability to concentrate in the context of their MD. Therefore, they were still included.
 
6
This was possible for 79% of LR participants. For the other 21% the second parent was not available for the interview, for example because participants grew up in a single parent household with little or no contact to the second parent. We still included those children and adolescents as excluding them would have decreased representativeness of the sample.
 
7
PVT: Data of 1 MD and 1 HR participant were unavailable due to technical difficulties.
 
8
As reaction times were not normally distributed, we repeated our analyses with bias scores calculated from median instead of mean reaction times as well as bias scores based on square root transformed reaction times. The pattern of results did not change.
 
9
As groups differed in psychopathology scores and dwell time, correlational analyses were repeated within each group: correlations between dwell time on sad faces and depressive symptoms as well as anxiety scores were non-significant (rs ≤ .28, ps ≥ .083).
 
10
Note that in our HR group, only children of depressed parents who did not meet criteria for any axis I disorder (current or past) were included, i.e. only the still mentally healthy. Therefore, this sample of HR children is not representative of unselected children of depressed parents, expected to show more pronounced psychopathology on average.
 
11
It has to be noted that dwell time on sad faces was not specifically related to depressive symptoms but also to anxiety scores.
 
Literatuur
5.
go back to reference Lo Iacono L, Carola V (2018) The impact of adolescent stress experiences on neurobiological development. Semin Cell Dev Biol 77:93–103CrossRef Lo Iacono L, Carola V (2018) The impact of adolescent stress experiences on neurobiological development. Semin Cell Dev Biol 77:93–103CrossRef
7.
go back to reference Beck AT, Haigh EA (2014) Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: The generic cognitive model. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 10:1–24CrossRef Beck AT, Haigh EA (2014) Advances in cognitive theory and therapy: The generic cognitive model. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 10:1–24CrossRef
9.
go back to reference Gotlib IH, Joormann J (2010) Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 6:285–312CrossRef Gotlib IH, Joormann J (2010) Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 6:285–312CrossRef
10.
go back to reference Mathews A, MacLeod C (2005) Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 1:167–195CrossRef Mathews A, MacLeod C (2005) Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 1:167–195CrossRef
12.
go back to reference De Raedt R, Koster EH (2010) Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: a reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 10(1):50–70CrossRef De Raedt R, Koster EH (2010) Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: a reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 10(1):50–70CrossRef
13.
go back to reference Armstrong T, Olatunji BO (2012) Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: a meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clin Psychol Rev 32(8):704–723CrossRef Armstrong T, Olatunji BO (2012) Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: a meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clin Psychol Rev 32(8):704–723CrossRef
14.
go back to reference Blakemore SJ, Choudhury S (2006) Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 47(3–4):296–312CrossRef Blakemore SJ, Choudhury S (2006) Development of the adolescent brain: implications for executive function and social cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 47(3–4):296–312CrossRef
15.
go back to reference Amso D, Scerif G (2015) The attentive brain: insights from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 16(10):606CrossRef Amso D, Scerif G (2015) The attentive brain: insights from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 16(10):606CrossRef
16.
go back to reference Lakdawalla Z, Hankin BL, Mermelstein R (2007) Cognitive theories of depression in children and adolescents: a conceptual and quantitative review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 10(1):1–24CrossRef Lakdawalla Z, Hankin BL, Mermelstein R (2007) Cognitive theories of depression in children and adolescents: a conceptual and quantitative review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 10(1):1–24CrossRef
17.
go back to reference Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN (2008) Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci 9(12):947–957CrossRef Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN (2008) Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci 9(12):947–957CrossRef
18.
go back to reference Platt B, Waters AM, Schulte-Koerne G, Engelmann L, Salemink E (2017) A review of cognitive biases in youth depression: attention, interpretation and memory. Cogn Emot 31(3):462–483CrossRef Platt B, Waters AM, Schulte-Koerne G, Engelmann L, Salemink E (2017) A review of cognitive biases in youth depression: attention, interpretation and memory. Cogn Emot 31(3):462–483CrossRef
20.
go back to reference Sylvester CM, Hudziak JJ, Gaffrey MS, Barch DM, Luby JL (2016) Stimulus-driven attention, threat bias, and sad bias in youth with a history of an anxiety disorder or depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol 44(2):219–231CrossRef Sylvester CM, Hudziak JJ, Gaffrey MS, Barch DM, Luby JL (2016) Stimulus-driven attention, threat bias, and sad bias in youth with a history of an anxiety disorder or depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol 44(2):219–231CrossRef
21.
go back to reference Harrison AJ, Gibb BE (2015) Attentional biases in currently depressed children: an eye-tracking study of biases in sustained attention to emotional stimuli. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 44(6):1008–1014CrossRef Harrison AJ, Gibb BE (2015) Attentional biases in currently depressed children: an eye-tracking study of biases in sustained attention to emotional stimuli. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 44(6):1008–1014CrossRef
22.
go back to reference Platt B, Murphy SE, Lau JY (2015) The association between negative attention biases and symptoms of depression in a community sample of adolescents. PeerJ 3:e1372CrossRef Platt B, Murphy SE, Lau JY (2015) The association between negative attention biases and symptoms of depression in a community sample of adolescents. PeerJ 3:e1372CrossRef
23.
go back to reference Neshat-Doost HT, Moradi AR, Taghavi MR, Yule W, Dalgleish T (2000) Lack of attentional bias for emotional information in clinically depressed children and adolescents on the dot probe task. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 41(3):363–368CrossRef Neshat-Doost HT, Moradi AR, Taghavi MR, Yule W, Dalgleish T (2000) Lack of attentional bias for emotional information in clinically depressed children and adolescents on the dot probe task. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 41(3):363–368CrossRef
25.
go back to reference MacLeod C, Mathews A, Tata P (1986) Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 95(1):15CrossRef MacLeod C, Mathews A, Tata P (1986) Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 95(1):15CrossRef
26.
go back to reference Schmukle SC (2005) Unreliability of the dot probe task. Eur J Pers 19:595–605CrossRef Schmukle SC (2005) Unreliability of the dot probe task. Eur J Pers 19:595–605CrossRef
29.
go back to reference De Voogd E, Wiers R, Prins P, Salemink E (2014) Visual search attentional bias modification reduced social phobia in adolescents. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 45(2):252–259CrossRef De Voogd E, Wiers R, Prins P, Salemink E (2014) Visual search attentional bias modification reduced social phobia in adolescents. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 45(2):252–259CrossRef
30.
go back to reference Aktar E, Van Bockstaele B, Pérez-Edgar K, Wiers RW, Bögels SM (2019) Intergenerational transmission of attentional bias and anxiety. Dev Sci 22(3):e12772CrossRef Aktar E, Van Bockstaele B, Pérez-Edgar K, Wiers RW, Bögels SM (2019) Intergenerational transmission of attentional bias and anxiety. Dev Sci 22(3):e12772CrossRef
31.
go back to reference Van Bockstaele B, Notebaert L, Salemink E, Clarke PJ, MacLeod C, Wiers RW, Bögels SM (2019) Effects of interpretation bias modification on unregulated and regulated emotional reactivity. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 64:123–132CrossRef Van Bockstaele B, Notebaert L, Salemink E, Clarke PJ, MacLeod C, Wiers RW, Bögels SM (2019) Effects of interpretation bias modification on unregulated and regulated emotional reactivity. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 64:123–132CrossRef
32.
go back to reference Högström J, Nordh M, Larson Lindal M, Taylor E, Serlachius E, Lundin Kleberg J (2019) Visual attention to emotional faces in adolescents with social anxiety disorder receiving cognitive behavioral therapy. PLoS ONE 14(11):e0225603CrossRef Högström J, Nordh M, Larson Lindal M, Taylor E, Serlachius E, Lundin Kleberg J (2019) Visual attention to emotional faces in adolescents with social anxiety disorder receiving cognitive behavioral therapy. PLoS ONE 14(11):e0225603CrossRef
34.
go back to reference Mogg K, Bradley B, Miles F, Dixon R (2004) Time course of attentional bias for threat scenes: testing the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis. Cogn Emot 18(5):689–700CrossRef Mogg K, Bradley B, Miles F, Dixon R (2004) Time course of attentional bias for threat scenes: testing the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis. Cogn Emot 18(5):689–700CrossRef
35.
go back to reference Lazarov A, Ben-Zion Z, Shamai D, Pine DS, Bar-Haim Y (2018) Free viewing of sad and happy faces in depression: a potential target for attention bias modification. J Affect Disord 238:94–100CrossRef Lazarov A, Ben-Zion Z, Shamai D, Pine DS, Bar-Haim Y (2018) Free viewing of sad and happy faces in depression: a potential target for attention bias modification. J Affect Disord 238:94–100CrossRef
36.
go back to reference Platt B, Sfärlea A, Buhl C, Loechner J, Neumüller J, Thomsen LA et al (2021) An eye-tracking study of attention biases in children at high familial risk for depression and their parents with depression. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 1–20 Platt B, Sfärlea A, Buhl C, Loechner J, Neumüller J, Thomsen LA et al (2021) An eye-tracking study of attention biases in children at high familial risk for depression and their parents with depression. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 1–20
37.
go back to reference Platt B (2017) The transmission of depression from parent to child: a trans-generational study of attention and interpretation biases (“GENERAIN”). Retrieved from https://www.osf.io/wuqg4 Platt B (2017) The transmission of depression from parent to child: a trans-generational study of attention and interpretation biases (“GENERAIN”). Retrieved from https://​www.​osf.​io/​wuqg4
38.
go back to reference Sfärlea A, Löchner J, Neumüller J, Asperud Thomsen L, Starman K, Salemink E et al (2019) Passing on the half-empty glass: a transgenerational study of interpretation biases in children at risk for depression and their parents with depression. J Abnorm Psychol 128(2):151–161. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000401CrossRef Sfärlea A, Löchner J, Neumüller J, Asperud Thomsen L, Starman K, Salemink E et al (2019) Passing on the half-empty glass: a transgenerational study of interpretation biases in children at risk for depression and their parents with depression. J Abnorm Psychol 128(2):151–161. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​abn0000401CrossRef
40.
go back to reference Joormann J, Talbot L, Gotlib IH (2007) Biased processing of emotional information in girls at risk for depression. J Abnorm Psychol 116(1):135CrossRef Joormann J, Talbot L, Gotlib IH (2007) Biased processing of emotional information in girls at risk for depression. J Abnorm Psychol 116(1):135CrossRef
41.
go back to reference Kujawa AJ, Torpey D, Kim J, Hajcak G, Rose S, Gotlib IH et al (2011) Attentional biases for emotional faces in young children of mothers with chronic or recurrent depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol 39(1):125–135CrossRef Kujawa AJ, Torpey D, Kim J, Hajcak G, Rose S, Gotlib IH et al (2011) Attentional biases for emotional faces in young children of mothers with chronic or recurrent depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol 39(1):125–135CrossRef
42.
go back to reference Montagner R, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Pine DS, Czykiel MS, Miguel EC et al (2016) Attentional bias to threat in children at-risk for emotional disorders: role of gender and type of maternal emotional disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 25(7):735–742CrossRef Montagner R, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Pine DS, Czykiel MS, Miguel EC et al (2016) Attentional bias to threat in children at-risk for emotional disorders: role of gender and type of maternal emotional disorder. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 25(7):735–742CrossRef
43.
go back to reference Gibb BE, Benas JS, Grassia M, McGeary J (2009) Children’s attentional biases and 5-HTTLPR genotype: potential mechanisms linking mother and child depression. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 38(3):415–426CrossRef Gibb BE, Benas JS, Grassia M, McGeary J (2009) Children’s attentional biases and 5-HTTLPR genotype: potential mechanisms linking mother and child depression. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 38(3):415–426CrossRef
44.
go back to reference Sfärlea A, Buhl C, Loechner J, Neumüller J, Thomsen LA, Starman K et al (2020) “I Am a Total… Loser”–the role of interpretation biases in youth depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol 48(10):1337–1350CrossRef Sfärlea A, Buhl C, Loechner J, Neumüller J, Thomsen LA, Starman K et al (2020) “I Am a Total… Loser”–the role of interpretation biases in youth depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol 48(10):1337–1350CrossRef
45.
go back to reference Asl AF, Ghanizadeh A, Mollazade J, Aflakseir A (2015) Differences of biased recall memory for emotional information among children and adolescents of mothers with MDD, children and adolescents with MDD, and normal controls. Psychiatry Res 228(2):223–227CrossRef Asl AF, Ghanizadeh A, Mollazade J, Aflakseir A (2015) Differences of biased recall memory for emotional information among children and adolescents of mothers with MDD, children and adolescents with MDD, and normal controls. Psychiatry Res 228(2):223–227CrossRef
46.
go back to reference Goodman SH, Gotlib IH (1999) Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: a developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychol Rev 106(3):458CrossRef Goodman SH, Gotlib IH (1999) Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: a developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychol Rev 106(3):458CrossRef
47.
go back to reference Everaert J, Duyck W, Koster EH (2014) Attention, interpretation, and memory biases in subclinical depression: a proof-of-principle test of the combined cognitive biases hypothesis. Emotion 14(2):331CrossRef Everaert J, Duyck W, Koster EH (2014) Attention, interpretation, and memory biases in subclinical depression: a proof-of-principle test of the combined cognitive biases hypothesis. Emotion 14(2):331CrossRef
49.
go back to reference DSM-IV-TR A (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. American Psychiatric Association Washington, DC. DSM-IV-TR A (2000) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. American Psychiatric Association Washington, DC.
50.
go back to reference Weiss RH (2006) CFT 20-R: Grundintelligenztest Skala 2 - Revision. Hogrefe, Göttingen Weiss RH (2006) CFT 20-R: Grundintelligenztest Skala 2 - Revision. Hogrefe, Göttingen
51.
go back to reference Schneider S, Unnewehr S, Margraf J (2009) Kinder-DIPS. Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes-und Jugendalter. Springer, Berlin Schneider S, Unnewehr S, Margraf J (2009) Kinder-DIPS. Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes-und Jugendalter. Springer, Berlin
52.
go back to reference Neuschwander M, In-Albon T, Adornetto C, Roth B, Schneider S (2013) Interrater-Reliabilität des Diagnostischen Interviews bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes-und Jugendalter (Kinder-DIPS). Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr 41(5):319–334CrossRef Neuschwander M, In-Albon T, Adornetto C, Roth B, Schneider S (2013) Interrater-Reliabilität des Diagnostischen Interviews bei psychischen Störungen im Kindes-und Jugendalter (Kinder-DIPS). Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr 41(5):319–334CrossRef
53.
go back to reference Schneider S, Suppiger A, DiNardo P, Barlow D, Becker E, Margraf J (2006) DIPS für DSM-IV-TR. Schneider S, Suppiger A, DiNardo P, Barlow D, Becker E, Margraf J (2006) DIPS für DSM-IV-TR.
54.
go back to reference In-Albon T, Suppiger A, Schlup B, Wendler S, Margraf J, Schneider S (2008) Validität des Diagnostischen Interviews bei psychischen Störungen (DIPS für DSM-IV-TR). Z Klin Psychol Psychother 37(1):33–42CrossRef In-Albon T, Suppiger A, Schlup B, Wendler S, Margraf J, Schneider S (2008) Validität des Diagnostischen Interviews bei psychischen Störungen (DIPS für DSM-IV-TR). Z Klin Psychol Psychother 37(1):33–42CrossRef
55.
go back to reference Hautzinger M, Keller F, Kühner C (2010) BDI-II. Klinisch-psychiatrische Ratingskalen für das Kindes-und Jugendalter 6:75, Hogrefe, Göttingen Hautzinger M, Keller F, Kühner C (2010) BDI-II. Klinisch-psychiatrische Ratingskalen für das Kindes-und Jugendalter 6:75, Hogrefe, Göttingen
56.
go back to reference Stiensmeier-Pelster J, Braune-Krickau M (2014) Schürmann M, Duda K. DIKJ. Depressionsinventar fü r Kinder und Jugendliche [Depression Inventory for Children and Adolescents]. Hogrefe, Göttingen, Germany. Stiensmeier-Pelster J, Braune-Krickau M (2014) Schürmann M, Duda K. DIKJ. Depressionsinventar fü r Kinder und Jugendliche [Depression Inventory for Children and Adolescents]. Hogrefe, Göttingen, Germany.
57.
go back to reference Unnewehr S, Joormann S, Schneider S, Margraf J (1992). Deutsche Übersetzung des state-trait anxiety inventory for children. Manuscript Drafting Process. Unnewehr S, Joormann S, Schneider S, Margraf J (1992). Deutsche Übersetzung des state-trait anxiety inventory for children. Manuscript Drafting Process.
58.
go back to reference Egger HL, Pine DS, Nelson E, Leibenluft E, Ernst M, Towbin KE et al (2011) The NIMH Child Emotional Faces Picture Set (NIMH-ChEFS): a new set of children’s facial emotion stimuli. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 20(3):145–156CrossRef Egger HL, Pine DS, Nelson E, Leibenluft E, Ernst M, Towbin KE et al (2011) The NIMH Child Emotional Faces Picture Set (NIMH-ChEFS): a new set of children’s facial emotion stimuli. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 20(3):145–156CrossRef
59.
go back to reference Klein AM, Salemink E, de Hullu E, Houtkamp E, Papa M, van der Molen M (2018) Cognitive bias modification reduces social anxiety symptoms in socially anxious adolescents with mild intellectual disabilities: a randomized controlled trial. J Autism Dev Disord 48(9):3116–3126CrossRef Klein AM, Salemink E, de Hullu E, Houtkamp E, Papa M, van der Molen M (2018) Cognitive bias modification reduces social anxiety symptoms in socially anxious adolescents with mild intellectual disabilities: a randomized controlled trial. J Autism Dev Disord 48(9):3116–3126CrossRef
61.
go back to reference Orquin JL, Holmqvist K (2018) Threats to the validity of eye-movement research in psychology. Behav Res Methods 50(4):1645–1656CrossRef Orquin JL, Holmqvist K (2018) Threats to the validity of eye-movement research in psychology. Behav Res Methods 50(4):1645–1656CrossRef
62.
go back to reference Skinner IW, Hübscher M, Moseley GL, Lee H, Wand BM, Traeger AC et al (2018) The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals. Behav Res Methods 50(5):1778–1792CrossRef Skinner IW, Hübscher M, Moseley GL, Lee H, Wand BM, Traeger AC et al (2018) The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals. Behav Res Methods 50(5):1778–1792CrossRef
63.
go back to reference Duque A, Vázquez C (2015) Double attention bias for positive and negative emotional faces in clinical depression: evidence from an eye-tracking study. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 46:107–114CrossRef Duque A, Vázquez C (2015) Double attention bias for positive and negative emotional faces in clinical depression: evidence from an eye-tracking study. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 46:107–114CrossRef
64.
go back to reference Allers R, Minkoff R, Hahn D, Mecchi I, Roberts J, Woolverton L et al (1994). The lion king. Walt Disney Pictures USA Allers R, Minkoff R, Hahn D, Mecchi I, Roberts J, Woolverton L et al (1994). The lion king. Walt Disney Pictures USA
66.
go back to reference Lang P (1980) Self-assessment manikin. The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL Lang P (1980) Self-assessment manikin. The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL
67.
go back to reference Murphy SE, Yiend J, Lester KJ, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ (2009) Short-term serotonergic but not noradrenergic antidepressant administration reduces attentional vigilance to threat in healthy volunteers. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 12(2):169–179CrossRef Murphy SE, Yiend J, Lester KJ, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ (2009) Short-term serotonergic but not noradrenergic antidepressant administration reduces attentional vigilance to threat in healthy volunteers. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 12(2):169–179CrossRef
68.
go back to reference Wells TT, Beevers CG (2010) Biased attention and dysphoria: manipulating selective attention reduces subsequent depressive symptoms. Cogn Emot 24(4):719–728CrossRef Wells TT, Beevers CG (2010) Biased attention and dysphoria: manipulating selective attention reduces subsequent depressive symptoms. Cogn Emot 24(4):719–728CrossRef
69.
go back to reference Miller GA, Chapman JP (2001) Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. J Abnorm Psychol 110(1):40CrossRef Miller GA, Chapman JP (2001) Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. J Abnorm Psychol 110(1):40CrossRef
70.
go back to reference Cracco E, Goossens L, Braet C (2017) Emotion regulation across childhood and adolescence: evidence for a maladaptive shift in adolescence. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 26(8):909–921CrossRef Cracco E, Goossens L, Braet C (2017) Emotion regulation across childhood and adolescence: evidence for a maladaptive shift in adolescence. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 26(8):909–921CrossRef
71.
go back to reference Zimmermann P, Iwanski A (2014) Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood: age differences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental variations. Int J Behav Med 38(2):182–194CrossRef Zimmermann P, Iwanski A (2014) Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood: age differences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental variations. Int J Behav Med 38(2):182–194CrossRef
72.
go back to reference Gross JL (2014) Emotion regulation: conceptual and empirical foundations. In: Gross JL (ed) The handbook of emotion regulation, 2nd edn. The Guildford Press, New York, pp 3–20 Gross JL (2014) Emotion regulation: conceptual and empirical foundations. In: Gross JL (ed) The handbook of emotion regulation, 2nd edn. The Guildford Press, New York, pp 3–20
73.
go back to reference Joormann J, Gotlib IH (2007) Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. J Abnorm Psychol 116(1):80CrossRef Joormann J, Gotlib IH (2007) Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. J Abnorm Psychol 116(1):80CrossRef
74.
75.
go back to reference Joormann J, Stanton CH (2016) Examining emotion regulation in depression: a review and future directions. Behav Res Ther 86:35–49CrossRef Joormann J, Stanton CH (2016) Examining emotion regulation in depression: a review and future directions. Behav Res Ther 86:35–49CrossRef
76.
go back to reference De Voogd EL, Wiers RW, Prins PJM, De Jong PJ, Boendermaker WJ, Zwitser RJ et al (2016) Online attentional bias modification training targeting anxiety and depression in unselected adolescents: short-and long-term effects of a randomized controlled trial. Behav Res Ther 87:11–22CrossRef De Voogd EL, Wiers RW, Prins PJM, De Jong PJ, Boendermaker WJ, Zwitser RJ et al (2016) Online attentional bias modification training targeting anxiety and depression in unselected adolescents: short-and long-term effects of a randomized controlled trial. Behav Res Ther 87:11–22CrossRef
77.
go back to reference Teachman BA, Joormann J, Steinman SA, Gotlib IH (2012) Automaticity in anxiety disorders and major depressive disorder. Clin Psychol Rev 32(6):575–603CrossRef Teachman BA, Joormann J, Steinman SA, Gotlib IH (2012) Automaticity in anxiety disorders and major depressive disorder. Clin Psychol Rev 32(6):575–603CrossRef
78.
go back to reference Pishyar R, Harris LM, Menzies RG (2008) Responsiveness of measures of attentional bias to clinical change in social phobia. Cogn Emot 22(7):1209–1227CrossRef Pishyar R, Harris LM, Menzies RG (2008) Responsiveness of measures of attentional bias to clinical change in social phobia. Cogn Emot 22(7):1209–1227CrossRef
80.
go back to reference Davis ML, Rosenfield D, Bernstein A, Zvielli A, Reinecke A, Beevers CG et al (2016) Attention bias dynamics and symptom severity during and following CBT for social anxiety disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol 84(9):795CrossRef Davis ML, Rosenfield D, Bernstein A, Zvielli A, Reinecke A, Beevers CG et al (2016) Attention bias dynamics and symptom severity during and following CBT for social anxiety disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol 84(9):795CrossRef
81.
go back to reference Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van Ijzendoorn MH (2007) Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull 133(1):1CrossRef Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van Ijzendoorn MH (2007) Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull 133(1):1CrossRef
83.
go back to reference Thapar A, Collishaw S, Pine DS, Thapar AK (2012) Depression in adolescence. Lancet 379(9820):1056–1067CrossRef Thapar A, Collishaw S, Pine DS, Thapar AK (2012) Depression in adolescence. Lancet 379(9820):1056–1067CrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
Biased Maintenance of Attention on Sad Faces in Clinically Depressed Youth: An Eye-Tracking Study
Auteurs
Christina Buhl
Anca Sfärlea
Johanna Loechner
Kornelija Starman-Wöhrle
Elske Salemink
Gerd Schulte-Körne
Belinda Platt
Publicatiedatum
02-09-2021
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Child Psychiatry & Human Development / Uitgave 1/2023
Print ISSN: 0009-398X
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3327
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-021-01229-z

Andere artikelen Uitgave 1/2023

Child Psychiatry & Human Development 1/2023 Naar de uitgave