Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Predictable chaos: a review of the effects of emotions on attention, memory and decision making

  • Review
  • Published:
Advances in Health Sciences Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Healthcare practice and education are highly emotional endeavors. While this is recognized by educators and researchers seeking to develop interventions aimed at improving wellness in health professionals and at providing them with skills to deal with emotional interpersonal situations, the field of health professions education has largely ignored the role that emotions play on cognitive processes. The purpose of this review is to provide an introduction to the broader field of emotions, with the goal of better understanding the integral relationship between emotions and cognitive processes. Individuals, at any given time, are in an emotional state. This emotional state influences how they perceive the world around them, what they recall from it, as well as the decisions they make. Rather than treating emotions as undesirable forces that wreak havoc on the rational being, the field of health professions education could be enriched by a greater understanding of how these emotions can shape cognitive processes in increasingly predictable ways.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arora, S., Ashrafian, H., Davis, R., Athanasiou, T., Darzi, A., & Sevdalis, N. (2010a). Emotional intelligence in medicine: A systematic review through the context of the ACGME competencies. Medical Education, 44(8), 749–764.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arora, S., Sevdalis, N., Aggarwal, R., Sirimanna, P., Darzi, A., & Kneebone, R. (2010b). Stress impairs psychomotor performance in novice laparoscopic surgeons. Surgical Endoscopy, 24(10), 2588–2593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin, E. J., Evans, P., Goldwater, R., & Potter, V. (2005). A preliminary study of emotional intelligence, empathy and exam performance in first year medical students. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(8), 1395–1405.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bacon, S. J. (1974). Arousal and the range of cue utilization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(1), 81–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspectives on psychological science, 1(1), 28–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bath, J., & Lawrence, P. F. (2012). Twelve tips for developing and implementing an effective surgical simulation programme. Medical Teacher, 34(3), 192–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). The somatic marker hypothesis: A neural theory of economic decision. Games and Economic Behavior, 52, 336–372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2000). Characterization of the decision-making deficit in patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 123, 2189–2202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernsten, D. (2002). Tunnel memories for autobiographical events: Central details are remembered ore frequently from shocking than from happy experiences. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1010–1020.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biss, R. K., & Hasher, L. (2011). Delighted and distracted: Positive affect increases priming for irrelevant information. Emotion, 11(6), 1474–1478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2003). Anxiety and the interpretation of ambiguous information: Beyond the emotion-congruent effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132(2), 294–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H. (2001). Mood and the use of general knowledge structures. In L. L. Martin (Ed.), Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s guidebook (pp. 9–26). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (2006). Mood and the regulation of information processing and behavior. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Hearts and minds: Affective influences on social cognition and behaviour (pp. 65–84). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Böhm, B., Rötting, N., Schwenk, W., Grebe, S., & Mansmann, U. (2001). A prospective randomized trial on heart rate variability of the surgical team during laparoscopic and conventional sigmoid resection. Archives of Surgery, 136(3), 305–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolte, A., Goschke, T., & Kuhl, J. (2003). Emotion and intuition effects of positive and negative mood on implicit judgments of semantic coherence. Psychological Science, 14(5), 416–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., & Coifman, K. (2004). The importance of being flexible the ability to both enhance and suppress emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological Science, 15(7), 482–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower. G. H., & Forgas, J. P. (2001). Mood and social memory. Handbook of affect and social cognition, 95–120.

  • Brancati, F. L. (1989). The art of pimping. Journal of the American Medical Association, 262, 89–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bulmer Smith, K., Profetto-McGrath, J., & Cummings, G. G. (2009). Emotional intelligence and nursing: An integrative literature review. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 46(12), 1624–1636.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (1995). A novel demonstration of enhanced memory associated with emotional arousal. Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 410–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (1998). Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 21, 294–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, H. A., Johannes, K., Poppenk, J. L., Moscovitch, M., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Evidence for the differential salience of disgust and fear in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 1100–1112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, M. G., Fletcher, I., O’Sullivan, H., & Dornan, T. (2014). Emotional intelligence in medical education: A critical review. Medical Education, 48(5), 468–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, R. Y., & Au, T. K. (2011). Nursing students’ anxiety and clinical performance. Journal of Nursing Education, 50(5), 286–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., & Palmer, J. (2009). Affective guidance of intelligent agents: How emotion controls cognition. Cognitive Systems Research, 10(1), 21–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clore, G. L., & Storbeck, J. (2006). Affect as information about liking, efficacy, and importance. In Forgas (Ed.), Affect in social thinking and behavior (pp. 123–142). New York, NY: Psychology press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colibazzi, T., Posner, J., Wang, Z., Gorman, D., Gerber, A., Yu, S., et al. (2010). Neural systems subserving valence and arousal during the experience of induced emotions. Emotion, 10(3), 377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Constans, J. I., & Mathews, A. M. (1993). Mood and the subjective risk of future events. Cognition and Emotion, 7(6), 545–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Croskerry, P. (2000). The cognitive imperative: Thinking about how we think. Academic Emergency Medicine, 7(11), 1223–1231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Croskerry, P. (2003a). The importance of cognitive errors in diagnosis and strategies to minimize them. Academic Medicine, 78(8), 775–780.

    Google Scholar 

  • Croskerry, P. (2003b). Cognitive forcing strategies in clinical decision making. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 41(1), 110–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Croskerry, P. (2009). Clinical cognition and diagnostic error: Applications of a dual process theory of reasoning. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 14(1), 27–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Custers, E. J. (2010). Long-term retention of basic science knowledge: A review study. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 15(1), 109–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Custers, E. J., Regehr, G., & Norman, G. R. (1996). Mental representations of medical diagnostic knowledge: A review. Academic Medicine, 71(Suppl. 10), S55–S61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Avon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1997). Towards a neuropathology of emotion and mood. Nature, 386, 769–770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., Everitt, B. J., & Bishop, D. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 351(1346), 1413–1420.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Neys, W. (2010). Heuristic bias, conflict, and rationality in decision-making. In Towards a theory of thinking (pp. 23–33). Berlin: Springer.

  • DeMaria, S., Bryson, E. O., Mooney, T. J., Silverstein, J. H., Reich, D. L., Bodian, C., et al. (2010). Adding emotional stressors to training in simulated cardiopulmonary arrest enhances participant performance. Medical Education, 44(10), 1006–1015.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeMaria, S., & Levine, A. I. (2013). The use of stress to enrich the simulated environment. In The comprehensive textbook of healthcare simulation (pp. 65–72). New York: Springer.

  • Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science, 298(5596), 1191–1194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dyrbye, L. N., Thomas, M. R., & Shanafelt, T. D. (2005). Medical student distress: Causes, consequences, and proposed solutions. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 80(12), 1613–1622).

  • Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. Handbook of affective sciences, 572–595.

  • Estrada, C. A., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1994). Positive affect improves creative problem solving and influences reported source of practice satisfaction in physicians. Motivation and Emotion, 18(4), 285–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estrada, C. A., Isen, A. M., & Young, M. J. (1997). Positive affect facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring in reasoning among physicians. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72(1), 117–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eva, K. W. (2005). What every teacher needs to know about clinical reasoning. Medical Education, 39, 98–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, K. (2001). Affective influences on social information processing. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), The handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 163–185). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischhoff, B., Gonzalez, R. M., Lerner, J. S., & Small, D. A. (2012). Evolving judgments of terror risks: Foresight, hindsight, and emotion: A reanalysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18, e1–e16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, J. H., Addis, D. R., & Giovanello, K. S. (2012). Differential effects of arousal in positive and negative autobiographical memories. Memory, 20(7), 771–778.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions-Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences. 1367–1378.

  • Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19(3), 313–332.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, B. H. (2010). Feelings and the body: The Jamesian perspective on autonomic specificity of emotion. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 383–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological Science, 19(5), 476–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The effects of low versus high approach-motivated positive affect on memory for peripherally versus centrally presented information. Emotion, 10(4), 599–603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gasper, K., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13(1), 34–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gendron, M., & Barrett, L. F. (2009). Reconstructing the past: A century of ideas about emotion in psychology. Emotion Review, 1(4), 316–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2008). The neural basis of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotions. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 577–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gros, C. (2010). Cognition and emotion: Perspectives on a closing gap. Cognitive Computation, 2(2), 78–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(9), 394–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, A., Bandiera, G., Nathens, A. B., & LeBlanc, V. R. (2012). The impact of stress on resident performance in simulated trauma scenarios. Journal of Trauma, 72(2), 497–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, A., Nathens, A. B., Bandiera, G. L., & LeBlanc, V. R. (2010). Threat and challenge: Cognitive appraisal and stress responses in simulated trauma resuscitations. Medical Education, 44(6), 587–594.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, J. P., Morey, R. A., Petty, C. M., Seth, S., Smoski, M. J., McCarthy, G., et al. (2010). Staying cool when things get hot: Emotion regulation modulates neural mechanisms of memory encoding. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hockey, G. R. J., John Maule, A., Clough, P. J., & Bdzola, L. (2000). Effects of negative mood states on risk in everyday decision making. Cognition and Emotion, 14(6), 823–855.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey-Murto, S., Leddy, J. J., Wood, T. J., Puddester, D., & Moineau, G. (2014). Does emotional intelligence at medical school admission predict future academic performance? Academic Medicine, 89(4), 638–643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M., Rosenzweig, A. S., & Young, M. J. (1991). The influence of positive affect on clinical problem solving. Medical Decision Making, 11(3), 221–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (2009). Emotion theory and research: Highlights, unanswered questions and emerging issues. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar (Straus and Giroux).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, R. L., Van Damme, I., & Levine, L. J. (2012). Motivation matters: Differing effects of pre-goal and post-goal emotions on attention and memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kensinger, E. A. (2004). Remembering emotional experiences: The contribution of valence and arousal. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 15(4), 241–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotions. Emotion Review, 1(2), 99–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Memory enhancement for emotional words: Are emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Memory & Cognition, 31, 1169–1180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2004). Two routes to emotional memory: Distinct neural processes for valence and arousal. Proceeding from the New York Academy of Sciences, 101(9), 3310–3315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kok, E. M., de Bruin, A. B., Robben, S. G., & van Merriënboer, J. J. (2012). Learning radiological appearances of diseases: Does comparison help? Learning and Instruction, 23, 90–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kromann, C. B., Jensen, M. L., & Ringsted, C. (2011). Test‐enhanced learning may be a gender‐related phenomenon explained by changes in cortisol level. Medical education, 45(2), 192–199.

  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion. American Psychologist, 46(4), 352–367.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeBlanc, V. R. (2009). The effects of acute stress on performance: Implications for health professions education. Academic Medicine, 84(10), S25–S33.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeBlanc, V. R., Regehr, C., Tavares, W., Scott, A. K., MacDonald, R., & King, K. (2012). The impact of stress on paramedic performance during simulated critical events. Prehospital and Disaster Medicine, 1(1), 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003). Effects for fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national field experiment. Psychological Science, 14, 144–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition and Emotion, 14(4), 473–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, J. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of the angry decision maker: How appraisal tendencies shape anger’s influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(2), 115–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libkumann, Tm., Stabler, C. L., & Otani, H. (2004). Arousal, valence, and memory for details. Memory, 12, 237–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macdougall, L., Martin, R., McCallum, I., & Grogan, E. (2013). Simulation and stress: acceptable to students and not confidence-busting. The Clinical Teacher, 10(1), 38–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maner, J. K., Richey, J. A., Cromer, K., Mallott, M., Lejuez, C. W., Joiner, T. E., et al. (2007). Dispositional anxiety and risk-avoidant decision-making. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(4), 665–675.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object based-framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 33–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConnell, M. M., & Eva, K. W. (2012). The role of emotion in the learning and transfer of clinical skills and knowledge. Academic Medicine, 87(10), 1316–1322.

    Google Scholar 

  • McConnell, M. M., & Shore, D. I. (2011). Upbeat and happy: Arousal as an important factor in studying attention. Cognition and Emotion, 25(7), 1184–1195.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNaughton, N. (2013). Discourse (s) of emotion within medical education: The ever-present absence. Medical Education, 47(1), 71–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mezirow, J. (2000). Learning as transformation: Critical perspectives on a theory in progress. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mittal, V., & Ross, W. T., Jr. (1998). The impact of positive and negative affect and issue framing on issue interpretation and risk taking. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76(3), 298–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monteiro, S. M., & Norman, G. (2013). Diagnostic reasoning: Where we’ve been, where we’re going. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 25(sup1), S26–S32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nabi, R. L. (2003). Exploring the framing effects of emotion: Do discrete emotions differentially influence information accessibility, information seeking, and policy preference? Communication Research, 30(2), 224–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naqvi, N., Shiv, B., & Bechar, A. (2006). The role of emotion on decision-making. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(5), 260–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P. M., & Setterlund, M. B. (1994). Emotion congruence in perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(4), 401–411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, G. R., & Eva, K. W. (2010). Diagnostic error and clinical reasoning. Medical Education, 44(1), 94–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(2), 148–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A. (2006). Emotion and cognition: Insights from studies on the human amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 27–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, J., Russell, J. A., & Peterson, B. S. (2005). The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 17(03), 715–734.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pottier, P., Dejoie, T., Hardouin, J. B., Le Loupp, A. G., Planchon, B., Bonnaud, A., et al. (2013). Effect of stress on clinical reasoning during simulated ambulatory consultations. Medical Teacher, 35(6), 472–480.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raghunathan, R., & Pham, M. T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal: Motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 79, 56–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Redelmeier, D. A. (2005). The cognitive psychology of missed diagnoses. Annals of Internal Medicine, 142(2), 115–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Regehr, C., LeBlanc, V. R., Jelley, R. B., & Barath, I. (2008). Acute stress and performance in police recruits. Stress and Health, 24(4), 295–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2004). Memory for emotional events. In D. Reisberg & P. Hertel (Eds.), Memory and emotions (pp. 3–41). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Remington, N. A., Fabrigar, L. R., & Visser, P. S. (2000). Reexamining the circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(2), 286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, A., French, C. C., Calder, A. J., Webb, B., Fox, R., & Young, A. W. (2002). Anxiety-related bias in the classification of emotionally ambiguous facial expressions. Emotion, 2(3), 273–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one’s cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(3), 410–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2006). Personality and emotional memory: How regulating emotion impairs memory for emotional events. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(5), 631–651.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roseman, I., & Evdokas, A. (2004). Appraisals cause experienced emotions: Experimental evidence. Cognition and Emotion, 18(1), 1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandars, J. (2009). The use of reflection in medical education: AMEE Guide No. 44. Medical Teacher, 31(8), 685–695.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satterfield, J. M., & Hughes, E. (2007). Emotion skills training for medical students: A systematic review. Medical Education, 41(10), 935–941.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K. R. (1994). Towards a concept of “modal emotions”. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 25–31). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and social behavior (pp. 44–62). Toronto: C.J. Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shanafelt, T. D., Boone, S., Tan, L., Dyrbye, L. N., Sotile, W., Satele, D., et al. (2012). Burnout and satisfaction with work-life balance among US physicians relative to the general US population. Archives of Internal Medicine, 172(18), 1377–1385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siemer, M., Mauss, I., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Same situation- different emotions: How appraisals shape our emotions. Emotion, 7(3), 592–600.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(6), 1460–1469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stolper, E., Van de Wiel, M., Van Royen, P., Van Bokhoven, M., Van der Weiiden, T., & Dinant, G. J. (2011). Gut feelings as a third track in general practitioners’ diagnostic reasoning. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 26(2), 197–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2008). Affective arousal as information: How affective arousal influences judgments, leaning, and memory. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1824–1843.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talarico, J. M., Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2009). Positive emotions enhance recall of peripheral details. Cognition and Emotion, 23(2), 380–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanner, C. A. (2006). Thinking like a nurse: A research-based model of clinical judgment in nursing. Journal of Nursing Education, 45(6), 204–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wear, D., Kokinova, M., Keck-McNulty, C., & Aultman, J. (2005). Pimping: Perspectives of 4th year medical students. Teaching and Learning in Medicine: An International Journal., 17, 184–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetzel, M., Black, S. A., Hanna, G. B., Athanasiou, T., Kneebone, R. L., Nestel, D., et al. (2010). The effects of stress and coping on surgical performance during simulations. Annals of Surgery, 251(1), 171–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woolley, A., & Kostopoulou, O. (2013). Clinical intuition in family medicine: More than first impressions. The annals of family medicine, 11(1), 60–66.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Vicki R. LeBlanc.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

LeBlanc, V.R., McConnell, M.M. & Monteiro, S.D. Predictable chaos: a review of the effects of emotions on attention, memory and decision making. Adv in Health Sci Educ 20, 265–282 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-014-9516-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-014-9516-6

Keywords

Navigation