Skip to main content
Log in

The activation of representative emotional verbal contexts interacts with vertical spatial axis

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Several experimental studies have shown that there exists an association between emotion words and the vertical spatial axis. However, the specific conditions under which this conceptual–physical interaction emerges are still unknown, and no study has been devised to test whether longer linguistic units than words can lead to a mapping of emotions on vertical space. In Experiment 1, Spanish and Colombian participants performed a representative verbal emotional contexts production task (RVEC task) requiring participants to produce RVEC for the emotions of joy, sadness, surprise, anger, fear, and disgust. The results showed gender and cultural differences regarding the average number of RVEC produced. The most representative contexts of joy and sadness obtained in Experiment 1 were used in Experiment 2 in a novel spatial–emotional congruency verification task (SECV task). After reading a sentence, the participants had to judge whether a probe word, displayed in either a high or low position on the screen, was congruent or incongruent with the previous sentence. The question was whether the emotion induced by the sentence could modulate the responses to the probes as a function of their position in a vertical axis by means of a metaphorical conceptual–spatial association. Overall, the results indicate that a mapping of emotions on vertical space can occur for linguistic units larger than words, but only when the task demands an explicit affective evaluation of the target.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Notes

  1. In the study reported herein, the focus is only on the number of verbal contexts since potential qualitative differences require different analyses that exceed the limits of this paper.

  2. Figure 2A might suggest that while there was a significant difference between males and women in the Colombian sample, this was not the case in the Spanish sample. Welch pairwise tests showed that those differences were significant in both cases [t(61.09) = 4.96, p < .001 (Colombiamale vs. females) and t(66.75) = 2.84, p = .005 (Spain male vs. females)], thus confirming our second hypothesis, i.e. women produce more RVEC than men. Additionally, a Welch pairwise test showed that there was no significant difference between Colombian and Spanish males t(48.08) = 1.44, p = .15) in Fig. 2a.

  3. We thank one of reviewers for suggesting this analysis.

References

  • Adolphs R (2001) The neurobiology of social cognition. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11(2):231–239

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Altarriba J, Bauer LM (2004) The distinctiveness of emotion concepts: a comparison between emotion, abstract, and concrete words. Am J Psychol 117(3):389–410

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ansorge U, Bohner G (2013) Investigating the association between valence and elevation with an implicit association task that requires upward and downward responding. Univ Psychol 12(5):1453–1471

    Google Scholar 

  • Ansorge U, Khalid S, König P (2013) Space-valence priming with subliminal and supraliminal words. Front Psychol 4:81. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00081

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ballesteros S, Millar S, Reales J (1998) Symmetry in haptic and visual shape perception. Percept Psychophys 60(3):389–404

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Basabe N, Paez D, Valencia J, Rimé B, Pennebaker J, Diener E, González JL (2000) Sociocultural factors predicting subjective experience of emotion. Psicothema 12:55–69

    Google Scholar 

  • Bhullar N (2013) Rating of intensity of emotions across auditory, visual, and auditory-visual sensory domains. Curr Psychol 32(2):186–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brebner J (2003) Gender and emotions. Pers Individ Differ 34(3):387–394

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brody LR, Hall JA (2008) Gender and emotion in context. In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Feldman-Barrett L (eds) Handbook of emotions, 3rd edn. Guilford Press, New York, pp 385–408

    Google Scholar 

  • Carou IF, Redondo J, Piñeiro A, Padrón I, Fernández-Rey J, Alcaraz M (2011) Attentional processing and recall of emotional words. Revista Latinoamericana de Psicología 43(3):401–418

    Google Scholar 

  • de la Vega I, Filippis M, Lachmair M, Dudschig C, Kaup B (2012) Emotional valence and physical space: limits of interaction. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 38(2):375–385

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elosúa MR, González A (1989) Una tipificación de contextos verbales afectivos. Rev de Psicología Universitas Tarraconensis 11(2):41–53

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández I, Carrera P, Sánchez F, Paez D, Candia L (2000) Differences between cultures in emotional verbal and non-verbal reactions. Psicothema 12(1):83–92

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernández I, Carrera P, Paez D, Sánchez F (2008) Interdependent self-construal, competitive attitudes, culture and emotional reactions on sadness. Psychologia 51:214–234

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer AH, Evers C (2010) Anger in the context of gender. In: Potegal M, Stemmler G, Spielberger CD (eds) Handbook of anger. Springer, New York, pp 349–360

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese V, Keysersa C, Rizzolatti G (2004) An unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 8(9):396–403

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg AM, Sato M, Cattaneo L (2008a) Use-induced motor plasticity affects the processing of abstract and concrete language. Curr Biol 18(7):R290–R291

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg AM, Sato M, Cattaneo L, Riggio L, Palumbo D, Buccino G (2008b) Processing abstract language modulates motor system activity. Q J Exp Psychol 61(6):905–919

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman A, de Vignemont F (2009) Is social cognition embodied? Trends Cogn Sci 13(4):154–159

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gozli DG, Chow A, Chasteen AL, Pratt J (2013) Valence and vertical space: saccade trajectory deviations reveal metaphorical spatial activation. Vis Cogn 21(5):628–646

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inglehart R, Foa R, Peterson P, Welzel C (2008) Development, freedom, and rising happiness. Perspect Psychol Sci 3(4):264–285

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacob P, Jeannerod M (2005) The motor theory of social cognition: a critique. Trends Cogn Sci 9(1):21–25

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ko SG, Lee TH, Yoon HY, Kwon JH, Mather M (2011) How does context affect assessments of facial emotion? The role of culture and age. Psychol Aging 26(1):48–59

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kousta ST, Vigliocco G, Vinson DP, Andrews M, Del Campo E (2011) The representation of abstract words: why emotion matters. J Exp Psychol Gen 140(1):14–34

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff G, Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Marmolejo-Ramos F, Dunn J (2013) On the activation of sensorimotor systems during the processing of emotionally-laden stimuli. Univ Psychol 12(5):1511–1542

    Google Scholar 

  • Marmolejo-Ramos F, Elosúa MR, Yamada Y, Hamm NF, Noguchi K (2013) Appraisal of space words and allocation of emotion words in bodily space. PLoS ONE 8(12):e81688. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0081688

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meier BP, Robinson MD (2004) Why the sunny side is up: associations between affect and vertical position. Psychol Sci 15(4):243–247

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meier BP, Robinson MD (2006) Does “feeling down” mean seeing down? Depressive symptoms and vertical selective attention. J Res Pers 40(4):451–461

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meier BP, Moller AC, Chen JJ, Riemer-Peltz M (2011) Spatial metaphor and real estate: north-south location biases housing preference. Soc Psychol Pers Sci 2(5):547–553

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meier BP, Schnall S, Schwarz N, Bargh JA (2012) Embodiment in social psychology. Top Cogn Sci 4(4):705–716

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meteyard L, Rodríguez S, Bahrami B, Vigliocco G (2012) Coming of age: a review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex 48:788–804

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milovchevich D, Howells K, Drew N, Day A (2001) Sex and gender role differences in anger: an Australian community study. Pers Individ Differ 31(2):117–127

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moseley R, Carota F, Hauk O, Mohr B, Pulvermüller F (2012) A role for the motor system in binding abstract emotional meaning. Cereb Cortex 22(7):1634–1647

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neel R, Becker DV, Neuberg SL, Kenrick DT (2012) Who expressed what emotion? Men grab anger, women grab happiness. J Exp Soc Psychol 48(2):583–586

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal PM, Barsalou LW, Winkielman P, Krauth-Gruber S, Ric F (2005) Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 9(3):184–211

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal PM, Winkielman P, Mondillon L, Vermeulen N (2009) Embodiment of emotion concepts. J Pers Soc Psychol 96(6):1120–1136

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Philippot P, Rimé B (1997) The perception of bodily sensations during emotion: a cross-cultural perspective. Pol J Bull 28(2):175–188

    Google Scholar 

  • Raposo A, Moss H, Stamatakis EA, Tyler LK (2009) Modulation of motor and premotor cortices by actions, action words, and action sentences. Neuropsychologia 47(2):388–396

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rimé B (2011) La compartición social de las emociones. Bilbao: Desclée de Brouwer. (Original Le partage social des émotions. Bruxelles: Mardaga, 2005)

  • Rosch E, Mervis CB, Gray WD, Johnson DM, Boyes-Braem P (1976) Basic objects in natural categories. Cogn Psychol 8(3):382–439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russell JA (1991) Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychol Bull 110:426–450

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Russell JA, Barrett LF (1999) Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: dissecting the elephant. J Pers Soc Psychol 76:805–819

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sakreida K, Scorolli C, Menz MM, Heim S, Borghi AM, Binkofski F (2013) Are abstract action words embodied? An fMRI investigation at the interface between language and motor cognition. Front Hum Neurosci 7:125. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00125

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Santiago J, Ouellet M, Román A, Valenzuela J (2012) Attentional factors in conceptual congruency. Cogn Sci 36(6):1051–1077

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Saxbe DE, Yang X-F, Borofsky LA, Immordino-Yang MH (2012) The embodiment of emotion: language use during the feeling of social emotions predicts cortical somatosensory activity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. doi:10.1093/scan/nss075

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaver P, Schwartz J, Kirson D, O’Connor C (1987) Emotion knowledge: further exploration of a prototype approach. J Pers Soc Psychol 52(6):1061–1086

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shields SA (2000) Thinking about gender, thinking about theory: gender and emotional experience. In: Fischer AH (ed) Gender and emotion: social psychological perspectives. Univesity Press & Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l’ Homme, Cambridge, pp 3–23

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Velasco C, Salgado-Montejo A, Marmolejo-Ramos F, Spence C (2014) Predictive packaging design: tasting shapes, typefaces, names, and sounds. Food Qual Prefer 34:88–95

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whelan R (2008) Effective analysis on reaction time tasks. Psychol Rec 58:475–782

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiemer-Hastings K, Xu X (2005) Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts. Cogn Sci 29(5):719–736

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zanolie K, van Dantzig S, Boot I, Wijnen J, Schubert TW, Giessner SR, Pecher D (2012) Mighty metaphors: behavioural and ERP evidence that power shifts attention on a vertical dimension. Brain Cogn 78(1):50–58

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Juan Botella (Autónoma University, Madrid) and Antonio Rodán (CEU University, Madrid) for providing facilities to take data for Experiment 1, Carlos Elias Cifuentes Villalobos (Los Libertadores University) for assisting with part of the analyses for Experiment 1, and Dr. Robyn Groves for assisting with proofreading this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Fernando Marmolejo-Ramos.

Additional information

This article is part of the Special Section on “Embodied Social Cognition”, guest-edited by Fernando Marmolejo Ramos and Amedeo Dangiulli.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Marmolejo-Ramos, F., Montoro, P.R., Elosúa, M.R. et al. The activation of representative emotional verbal contexts interacts with vertical spatial axis. Cogn Process 15, 253–267 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-014-0620-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-014-0620-6

Keywords

Navigation