Skip to main content
Log in

Synchronising movements with the sounds of a virtual partner enhances partner likeability

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Previous studies have demonstrated that synchronising movements with other people can influence affiliative behaviour towards them. While research has focused on synchronisation with visually observed movement, synchronisation with a partner who is heard may have similar effects. We replicate findings showing that synchronisation can influence ratings of likeability of a partner, but demonstrate that this is possible with virtual interaction, involving a video of a partner. Participants performed instructed synchrony in time to sounds instead of the observable actions of another person. Results show significantly higher ratings of likeability of a partner after moving at the same time as sounds attributed to that partner, compared with moving in between sounds. Objectively quantified synchrony also correlated with ratings of likeability. Belief that sounds were made by another person was manipulated in Experiment 2, and results demonstrate that when sounds are attributed to a computer, ratings of likeability are not affected by moving in or out of time. These findings demonstrate that interaction with sound can be experienced as social interaction in the absence of genuine interpersonal contact, which may help explain why people enjoy engaging with recorded music.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Notes

  1. In conditions where participants had been told not to synchronise, this was preceded by “Given that your aim was not to synchronise,”.

  2. Following null results relating to the change in virtual eye gaze in Experiment 2, further analyses were performed on the eye tracking data (on pupil dilation, variability of gaze within and outside eye region of image, and time course of eye gaze change) to determine whether an effect was being masked by the virtual eye contact measure. These did not reveal any reliable differences between the “with” and “between” conditions. A third experiment was conducted as a replication of Experiment 1 using the minimum participants statistically required to demonstrate the effect identified in Experiment 1. As in Experiment 2, these results did not reveal any significant differences between eye gaze in the two instruction conditions and demonstrated a non-significant trend towards increases in eye gaze being associated with rating a partner as less likeable.

References

  • Baron-Cohen S (1997) Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Buccino G, Binkofski F, Fink GR, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Seitz RJ, Zilles K, Rizzolatti G, Freund HJ (2001) Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci 13(2):400–404

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand TL, Bargh JA (1999) The Chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and social interaction. J Pers Soc Psychol 76(6):893–910

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand TL, Maddux WW, Lakin JL (2005) Beyond the perception-behavior link: the ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry. In: Hassin RR, Uleman JS, Bargh JA (eds) The new unconscious. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 334–361

    Google Scholar 

  • Demos AP, Chaffin R, Begosh KT, Daniels JR, Marsh KL (2012) Rocking to the beat: effects of music and partner’s movements on spontaneous interpersonal coordination. J Exp Psychol Gen 141(1):49–53

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eitan Z, Granot RY (2006) How music moves. Music Percept 23(3):221–248

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engel LR, Frum C, Puce A, Walker NA, Lewis JW (2009) Different categories of living and non-living sound-sources activate distinct cortical networks. Neuroimage 47(4):1778–1791

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Pavesi G, Rizzolatti G (1995) Motor facilitation during action observation: a magnetic stimulation study. J Neurophysiol 73(6):2608

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Filippin L (2011) T2T Package. http://psy.ck.sissa.it/t2t/About_T2T.html

  • Freedberg D, Gallese V (2007) Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience. Trends Cogn Sci 11(5):197–203

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzola V, Aziz-Zadeh L, Keysers C (2006) Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Curr Biol 16(18):1824–1829

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guastella AJ, Mitchell PB, Dadds MR (2008a) Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces. Biol Psychiat 63(1):3–5

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guastella AJ, Mitchell PB, Mathews F (2008b) Oxytocin enhances the encoding of positive social memories in humans. Biol Psychiat 64(3):256–258

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hagen EH, Bryant GA (2003) Music and dance as a coalition signaling system. Hum Nat Int Bios 14(1):21–51

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hove MJ, Risen JL (2009) It’s all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc Cogn 27(6):949–960

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni M (2005) Neural mechanisms of imitation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15(6):632–637

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Isen AM (1970) Success, failure, attention, and reaction to others: the warm glow of success. J Pers Soc Psychol 15(4):294–301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Issartel J, Marin L, Cadopi M (2007) Unintended interpersonal co-ordination: “Can we march to the beat of our own drum?”. Neurosci Lett 411(3):174–179

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner S, Tomasello M (2009) Joint drumming: social context facilitates synchronization in preschool children. J Exp Child Psychol 102(3):299–314

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinke CL (1986) Gaze and eye contact: a research review. Psychol Bull 100(1):78–100

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kokal I, Engel A, Kirschner S, Keysers C (2011) Synchronized drumming enhances activity in the caudate and facilitates prosocial commitment-if the rhythm comes easy. PLoS ONE 6(11):e27272. doi:10.21371/journal.pone.0027272

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Konvalinka I, Vuust P, Roepstorff A, Frith CD (2010) Follow you, follow me: continuous mutual prediction and adaptation in joint tapping. Q J Exp Psychol 63(11):2220–2230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaFrance M (1979) Nonverbal synchrony and rapport—analysis by the cross-lag panel technique. Soc Psychol 42(1):66–70

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakens D (2010) Movement synchrony and perceived entitativity. J Exp Soc Psychol 46(5):701–708

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin JL, Jefferis VE, Cheng CM, Chartrand TL (2003) The chameleon effect as social glue: evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. J Nonverbal Behav 27(3):145–162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Launay J, Dean RT, Bailes F (2013) Synchronization can influence trust following virtual interaction. Exp Psychol 60(1):53

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Madison G, Merker B (2002) On the limits of anisochrony in pulse attribution. Psychol Res 66(3):201–207

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mardia KV, Jupp PE (2000) Directional statistics. Wiley, Chichester

    Google Scholar 

  • Miles LK, Griffiths JL, Richardson MJ, Macrae CN (2009a) Too late to coordinate: contextual influences on behavioral synchrony. Eur J Soc Psychol 40(1):52–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Miles LK, Nind LK, Macrae CN (2009b) The rhythm of rapport: interpersonal synchrony and social perception. J Exp Soc Psychol 45(3):585–589

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miles LK, Lumsden J, Richardson MJ, Macrae NC (2011) Do birds of a feather move together? Group membership and behavioral synchrony. Exp Brain Res 211(3–4):495–503

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Modigliani A (1971) Embarrassment, facework, and eye contact: testing a theory of embarrassment. J Pers Soc Psychol 17(1):15–24

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Moukheiber A, Rautureau G, Perez-Diaz F, Soussignan R, Dubal S, Jouvent R, Pelissolo A (2009) Gaze avoidance in social phobia: objective measure and correlates. Behav Res Ther 48(2):147–151

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ollen J (2006) A criterion-related validity test of selected indicators of musical sophistication using expert ratings. http://www.ohiolink.edu/etd/view.cgi?osu1161705351. The Ohio State University

  • Oullier O, De Guzman GC, Jantzen KJ, Lagarde J, Kelso JAS (2008) Social coordination dynamics: measuring human bonding. Soc Neurosci 3(2):178–192

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Overy K, Molnar-Szakacs I (2009) Being together in time: musical experience and the mirror neuron system. Music Percept 26(5):489–504

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinkham AE, Hopfinger JB, Pelphrey KA, Piven J, Penn DL (2008) Neural bases for impaired social cognition in schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorders. Schizophr Res 99(1–3):164–175

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G (2005) The mirror neuron system and its function in humans. Anat Embryol 210(5):419–421

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz N, Bekkering H, Knoblich G (2006) Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn Sci 10(2):70–76

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semin GR, Cacioppo JT (2008) Grounding social cognition: synchronization, coordination and co-regulation. In: Semin GR, Smith ER (eds) Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective and neuroscientific approaches. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 119–147

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Shimojo S, Simion C, Shimojo E, Scheier C (2003) Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference. Nat Neurosci 6(12):1317–1322

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sommerville JA, Decety J (2006) Weaving the fabric of social interaction: articulating developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience in the domain of motor cognition. Psychon Bull Rev 13(2):179–200

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steinbeis N, Koelsch S (2009) Understanding the intentions behind man-made products elicits neural activity in areas dedicated to mental state attribution. Cereb Cortex 19(3):619–623

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tognoli E, Lagarde J, DeGuzman GC, Kelso JAS (2007) The phi complex as a neuromarker of human social coordination. P Natl Acad Sci USA 104(19):8190–8195

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Valdesolo P, DeSteno D (2011) Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion 11(2):262–266

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins K, Strafella A, Paus T (2003) Seeing and hearing speech excites the motor system involved in speech production. Neuropsychologia 41(8):989–994

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltermuth SS (2012) Synchrony and destructive obedience. Soc Influ 7(2):78–89

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltermuth SS, Heath C (2009) Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol Sci 20(1):1–5

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jacques Launay.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Launay, J., Dean, R.T. & Bailes, F. Synchronising movements with the sounds of a virtual partner enhances partner likeability. Cogn Process 15, 491–501 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-014-0618-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-014-0618-0

Keywords

Navigation