Skip to main content
Log in

Coping with uncertainty: police strategies for resilient decision-making and action implementation

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Cognition, Technology & Work Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study uses a hostage negotiation setting to demonstrate how a team of strategic police officers can utilize specific coping strategies to minimize uncertainty at different stages of their decision-making in order to foster resilient decision-making to effectively manage a high-risk critical incident. The presented model extends the existing research on coping with uncertainty by (1) applying the RAWFS heuristic (Lipshitz and Strauss in Organ Behav Human Decis Process 69:149–163, 1997) of individual decision-making under uncertainty to a team critical incident decision-making domain; (2) testing the use of various coping strategies during “in situ” team decision-making by using a live simulated hostage negotiation exercise; and (3) including an additional coping strategy (“reflection-in-action”; Schön in The reflective practitioner: how professionals think in action. Temple Smith, London, 1983) that aids naturalistic team decision-making. The data for this study were derived from a videoed strategic command meeting held within a simulated live hostage training event; these video data were coded along three themes: (1) decision phase; (2) uncertainty management strategy; and (3) decision implemented or omitted. Results illustrate that, when assessing dynamic and high-risk situations, teams of police officers cope with uncertainty by relying on “reduction” strategies to seek additional information and iteratively update these assessments using “reflection-in-action” (Schön 1983) based on previous experience. They subsequently progress to a plan formulation phase and use “assumption-based reasoning” techniques in order to mentally simulate their intended courses of action (Klein et al. 2007), and identify a preferred formulated strategy through “weighing the pros and cons” of each option. In the unlikely event that uncertainty persists to the plan execution phase, it is managed by “reduction” in the form of relying on plans and standard operating procedures or by “forestalling” and intentionally deferring the decision while contingency planning for worst-case scenarios.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alison L, Eyre M, Humann M (2010) Losing sight of the ‘golden mean’: accountogenic decisions in UK policing. In: Mosier K, Fischer U (eds) Knowledge management: Expert performance in complex situations. Taylor & Francis, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Allaire Y, Firsirotu M (1989) How to implement radical strategies in large organisations. In: Tushman ML (ed) The management of organizations. Harper Collins, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson CJ (2003) The psychology of doing nothing: forms of decision avoidance result from reason and emotion. Psychol Bull 129:139–167

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arai D (1997) Temporal resolution of uncertainty in risky choices. Acta Psychol 96:15–26

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker SM, Middleton SA (2008) Improving hospital preparedness for radiological terrorism: perspectives from emergency department physicians and nurses. Disaster Med Public Health Prep 2(3):174–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger CR, Calabrese RJ (1975) Some exploration in initial interaction and beyond: toward a developmental theory of communication. Hum Commun Res 1:99–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein P (1998) Against the gods: the remarkable story of risk. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradac JJ (2001) Theory comparison: uncertainty reduction, problematic integration, uncertainty management, and other curious constructs. J Commun 51(3):477–497

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camerer CF, Weber M (1992) Recent developments in modelling preferences: uncertainty and ambiguity. J Risk Uncert 5:325–370

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen MS (1989) A database tool to support probabilistic assumption-based reasoning in intelligence analysis. In: Proceedings of the 1989 joint director of the C2 symposium, Ft. M cNair, VA, June 27–29

  • Cohen MS (2010) Knowns, known unknowns, and unknown unknowns. In: Mosier K, Fischer U (eds) Informed by knowledge: expert performance in complex situations. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 371–393

    Google Scholar 

  • Crego J, Alison L (2004) Control and legacy as functions of perceived criticality in major incidents. J Investig Psychol Offend Prof 1:207–225

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crichton M, McGeorge P, Flin R (2007) Decision making by operational incident commanders: decision strategy selection. In: Noyes J (ed) Decision making in complex environments. Ashgate, Aldershot

    Google Scholar 

  • Denning R, Bennett S, Mazerolle L, Stocks B (2009) Serious violent crime investigations: a systematic literature search and technical report prepared for the National Police Improvement Agency. Centre Of excellence in Policing and Security, July 2009

  • Dhar R (1997) Consumer preference for a no-choice option. J Consumer Res 24(2):215–231

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiBonaventura MC, Chapman GB (2008) Do decision biases predict bad decisions? Omission bias, naturalness bias and influenza vaccination. Med Decis Making 28(4):532–539

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus HL, Dreyfus SE (1986) Five steps from novice to expert. In: Dreyfus HL, Dreyfus SE, Athanasiou T (eds) Mind over machine: the power of intuition and expertise in the era of the computer. Free Press, New York, pp 16–52

    Google Scholar 

  • Engle RW (2007) Integrative comments working memory: the mind is richer than the models. In: Roediger HL, Dudal Y, Fitzpatrick SM (eds) Science of memory: concepts. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Eraut M (2000) The intuitive practitioner: a critical overview. In: Atkinson T, Claxton G (eds) The intuitive practitioner. Open University Press, Buckingham

    Google Scholar 

  • Fellows LK (2004) The cognitive neuroscience of human decision making: a review and conceptual framework. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 3:159–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger L (1957) A theory of cognitive dissonance. Row, Peterson, Evanston

    Google Scholar 

  • Fiore SM, Rosen MA, Smith-Jentsch KA, Salas E, Letsky M, Warner N (2010) Toward an understanding of macrocognition in teams: predicting processes in complex collaborative contexts. Hum Factors 52(2):203–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • French JA, Kamil AC, Leger DW (2001) Evolutionary psychology and motivation. University of Nebraska Press, Nebraska

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman VJ, Lipshitz R (1992) Teaching people to shift cognitive gears: overcoming resistance on the road to model II. J Appl Behav Res 28(1):118–136

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Froot KA, Scharfstein DS, Stein JC (1994) A framework for risk management. J Appl Corp Finan 7(3):22–32

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • George AL (1980) Presidential decision making in foreign policy: the effective use of information and advice. Westview Press, Boulder

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith M, Koriat A, Pansky A (2005) Strategic regulation of grain size in memory reporting over time. J Mem Lang 52:505–525

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamm RM (1988) Clinical intuition and clinical analysis: expertise and the cognitive continuum. In: Dowey J, Elstein A (eds) Professional judgement: a reader in clinical decision making. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Hansson O (1996) Decision making under great uncertainty. Philos Soc Sci 26:369–386

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hastie R (2001) Problems for judgment and decision making. Annu Rev Psychol 52:653–683

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath C, Gonzalez R (1995) Interaction with others increases decision confidence but not decision quality: evidence against information collection views of interactive decision-making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 61:305–326

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D, Frederick S (2002) Representativeness revisited: attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In: Gilovich T, Griffin D, Kahneman D (eds) Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 49–81

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman D, Klein G (2009) Conditions for intuitive expertise: a failure to disagree. Am Psychol 64(6):515–526

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein GA (1993) A recognition-primed decision (RPD) model of rapid decision making. In: Klein GA, Orasanu J, Calderwood R, Zsambok CE (eds) Decision making in action: models and methods. Ablex Publishing Corp, Norwood, pp 138–147

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein G, Pin CL, Snowdon D (2007) Anticipatory thinking. In: Proceedings of the eighth international NDM conference, Pacific Grove, CA, June 2007

  • Kobus DA, Proctor S, Holste S (2001) Effects of experience and uncertainty during dynamic decision making. Int J Ind Ergon 28(5):275–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krikelas J (1983) Information seeking behaviour: patterns and concepts. Drexel Lib Q 19(2):5–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski AW, Thompson EP (1999) The illusory second mode, or the Cue is the Message. Psychol Inq 10(2):182–193

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski AW, Webster DM (1996) Motivated closing of the mind: “seizing” and “freezing.”. Psychol Rev 103(2):263–283

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krupinsky EA, Tillack AA, Richter L, Henderson JT, Bhattacharyya AK, Scott KM et al (2006) Eye-movement study and human performance using telepathy and differences with experience. Hum Pathol 37:1543–1556

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipshitz R (1997) Coping with uncertainty: beyond the reduce, quantify and plug heuristic. In: Flin R, Sala E, Strub M, Martin L (eds) Decision making under stress: emerging themes and applications. Ashgate, Aldershot

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipshitz R, Strauss O (1997) Coping with uncertainty: a naturalistic decision making analysis. Organ Behav Human Decis Process 69:149–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipshitz R, Klein G, Orasanu J, Salas R (2001) Taking stock of naturalistic decision making. Behav Decis Making 14(5):331–352

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipshitz R, Omodei M, McLennan J, Wearing A (2007) What’s burning? The RAWFS heuristic on the fire ground. In: Hoffman R (ed) Expertise out of context. Lawrence Erlbaum, New Jersey

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann L, Burnett P, Radford M, Ford S (1997) The Melbourne decision making questionnaire: an instrument for measuring patterns for coping with decisional conflict. J Behav Decis Making 10(1):1–19

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKenzie J, Woolf N, van Winkelen C, Morgan C (2009) Cognition in strategic decision making: a model of non-conventional thinking capacities for complex situations. Manag Decis 47(2):209–232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nohrstedt D (2000) Intuition and avoidance in foreign policy crises: how decisionmakers cope with uncertainty. Masters thesis. Department of Peace and Conflict Research, Uppsala University, Uppsala

  • Nonaka I (1994) A dynamic theory of organizational knowledge creation. Organ Sci 5(1):14–37

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orlitzky M, Hirokawa RY (2001) To error is human, to correct for it divine: a meta-analysis of research testing the functional theory of group decision-making effectiveness. Small Group Res 32(3):313–341

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osman M (2010) Controlling uncertainty: a review of human behaviour in complex dynamic environments. Psychol Bull 136:65–86

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington N, Hastie R (1988) Explanation-based decision making: the effects of memory structure on judgement. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 14:521–533

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pornpitakpan C (2006) The persuasiveness of source credibility: a critical review of five decades’ evidence. J Appl Soc Psychol 34:243–281

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Porter M, Millar VE (1985) How information gives you competitive advantage. Harvard Bus Rev 63(4):149–160

    Google Scholar 

  • Raiffa H (1968) Decision analysis: introductory lectures on choices under uncertainty. Addison Wesley, Reading

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Rake EL, Njå O (2009) Perceptions and performances of experienced incident commanders. J Risk Res 12(5):665–685

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reimer T, Mata R, Stoecklin M (2004) The use of heuristics in persuasion: deriving cues on source expertise from argument quality. Curr Res Soc Psychol 10(6):69–84

    Google Scholar 

  • Renkl A, Hilbert T, Schworm S (2008) Example based learning in heuristic domains: a cognitive load theory account. Educ Psychol Rev 21:67–78

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reyna VF, Lloyd FJ (2006) Physician decision-making and cardiac risk: effects of knowledge, risk perception, risk tolerance, and fuzzy processing. J Exp Psychol Appl 12:179–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Romano SJ, McMann MF (eds) (1997) Crisis negotiations: a compendium. Crisis negotiation unit, critical incident response group. Quantico, VA7 FBI academy

  • Ross KG, Klein GA, Thunholm P, Schmitt JF, Baxter HC (2004) The recognition primed decision model. Military Rev (July–Aug, pp 6–10)

  • Schatz S, Dolletski-Lazar R, Colombo G et al. (2011) Advanced perceptual skills: improving their training, evaluation and operational effectiveness. Presented at the 10th international conference on naturalistic decision making, Orlando, FL

  • Schön DA (1983) The reflective practitioner: how professionals think in action. Temple Smith, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Souchon N, Cabagno G, Traclet A, Trouilloud D, Maio G (2009) Referees’ use of heuristics: the moderating impact of standard of competition. J Sports Sci 27(7):695–700

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thunholm P (2003) Military planning and decision making under time-pressure: a scenario based experimental comparison between two models. In: Thunholm P (ed) Military decision making and planning: towards a new prescriptive model. Doctoral dissertation at Stockholm University. Akademitryck, Edsbruk

  • Tversky A, Kahneman D (1974) Judgement under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 185:1124–1131

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Heuvel C, Alison L, Crego J (2011) How uncertainty and accountability can derail strategic ‘save life’ decisions in counter-terrorism simulations: a descriptive model of choice deferral and omission bias. J Behav Decis Making. doi:10.1002/bdm.723

  • Vecchi GM, Van Hasselt VB, Romano SJ (2005) Crisis (hostage) negotiation: current strategies and issues in high-risk conflict resolution. Aggress Viol Behav 10:533–551

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weick K, Sutcliffe K (2001) Managing the unexpected: assuring high performance in an age of complexity. Jossey-Bass, Sutcliffe

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetzel CM, Kneebone RL, Woloshynowych M, Nestel D, Moorthy K, Kidd J, Darzi A (2006) The effect of stress on surgical performance. Am J Surg 191:5–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wickens CD, Hollands JG (2000) Engineering psychology and human performance. Prentice Hall, New York

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laurence Alison.

Appendices

Appendix 1: Coding guidelines

Coders were presented with a transcript of the discussions between decision-makers during the hostage negotiation simulation. They were also given the following guidelines with regard to coding the discourse taking place during the situation. There were three forms of coding: (1) coding the decision phase; (2) coding the uncertainty coping strategy used; and (3) coding the decision as “made” or “omitted”.

1.1 Decision phase coding

 

Decision phase (code)

Description

Situation assessment (SA)

Participants are creating a storyboard or an image of what is going on at that moment in time (e.g. what is going on? What do I perceive?)

Plan formulation (PF)

Participants discuss strategies for dealing with the dynamic event

They formulate options/hypotheses on how to deal with situation

They define the roles/responsibilities required to carry out strategy

Plan execution (PE)

Participants create tactics for carrying out a previously defined plan

They discuss the physical actions required to execute previously formulated plans

They assign the roles/responsibilities to carry out tactical action

1.2 Coping with uncertainty strategy codes

 

Uncertainty coping strategy (code):

Description of how each strategy and tactics to achieve this:

Reduction (R)

Seeking further information to reduce uncertainty via the tactics of

 Advice seeking (from external sources and each other)

 Extrapolating meaning from information at hand—for example, “if the offender is saying X, it means he might be inclined to do Y”

 Discussing standard operating procedures, policy, and plans

 Redundant deliberation—asking repeated questions, not making any progress in their thinking, planning, or decision-making

Assumption-based reasoning (AbR)

Constructing assumption-based hypotheses to reduce uncertainty via the tactics of

 Mental simulation (create hypothetical mental models and simulating the steps of their plans if they were to implement them)

 Mental rehearsal (testing hypothetical mental model by imagining possible consequences if they were to implement a plan or strategy)

 Anticipatory thinking (focusing on the potential outcome of their actions in both short term and long term)

Weighing pros and cons (WPC)

Listing and trading off the pros and cons of possible courses of actions to overcome uncertainty

Forestalling (F)

Remove uncertainty by avoiding non-reversible actions through

 Preparing for worst-case scenario: identifying various contingency plans that may be used in case offender does something extreme (e.g. becomes violent to the victims, tries to escape)

 Intentional deferral: an active choice to not make a decision yet, with explicit intention to revisit this decision at a later point in time

Suppression (S)

Taking a carelessly calculated gamble to reduce uncertainty via the tactics of

 Denial of uncertainty (either knowingly or unknowingly)

 Acknowledging uncertainty but not doing anything about it

Reflection-in-action (RIA)

Process of continually critiquing and revising assumptions via

 Reflecting on previously chosen actions; for example, “before when the offender said X, we said/did Y. That resulted in Z”

 Reframing their mental models; for example, “I thought the offender was X but this indicates he might be Y”

 An incremental and deliberate monologue describing the dynamic environment; for example, “I think what the offender is now feeling/doing is…”

1.3 Decision made versus omitted

Every discussion should be given one decision “Made” or “Omitted” code at the end of that discussion. The discussion will only be coded for a decision “made” if the team has explicitly identified a decision and tasked someone with implementing that choice; for example, “Great, on your advice then, the decision is to provide the offender with food”.

Appendix 2: Example quotes of coping strategies coded during the SA phase

 

Coping strategy

Tactic within strategy

Description of strategy in HN context

Quote

Reduction (initial SA)

Active information search

The IC and NC often sought intelligence, information, and advice from each other in order to inform their understanding of the dynamic situation

OK, run me through, where are we going to take this to? Stage 1: if I said to you, “I’m putting negotiators in there (if I am explaining this to someone else) what do I expect to get in the next half hour?”

Assumption-based reasoning (initial SA)

Conjecturing

Officers made assumptions about what was occurring inside the stronghold (when insufficient information was available) to create a working understanding of the incident

“I don’t think it is reality that they would give up all the hostages because if they do give up all the hostages then they’ve given up bargaining chips. My view on these people is that the reason why they have the hostages is to buy them time, to mentally think about where they are and to escape. To give up all the hostages is like giving themselves up, so I don’t think that as a criminal I would be releasing hostages on that.”

Reflection-in-action (iterative SA)

Stepping back and reflecting

The NC and IC discussed previous actions in order to make an informed decision

IC: “right, what have we done for them?”

NC: “we’ve swapped over negotiators when they asked us to. We haven’t told them any lies, we have been engaging with them […] we have engaged with them, been nice to them […] haven’t gone crashing in, haven’t told them any lies…”

IC: “well, can’t we use all that and say ‘right we have done all that, what are you now going to do for us?’”

Suppression (maladaptive SA)

Decision omission

Discussion over whether to introduce a “reality check” to the offenders (by making consequences clear to the offenders), was omitted as the NC attempted to suppress high levels of uncertainty.

IC: “so why are you, if it is 50–50, why are you advising to go with the reality check, or you just saying that’s an option?”

NC: “I’m just saying that’s an option (shrugs)”

Appendix 2.1: Example quotes of coping strategies coded during the PF phase

 

Coping strategy

Tactic within strategy

Description of strategy in HN context

Quote

Assumption-based reasoning

Mental simulation

The IC used mental simulation to plan the potential processes of action

“I don’t want to get into choosing, if they start giving me names and saying “you choose” I’m not going to do that. They’ve […] given me an opening bid, and I sort of want it to be known that I am interested in that opening bid. I don’t know what words you’re going to use as a negotiator but I am interested… but here is my counter bid, let the 3 go and the car will get to you”

Mental rehearsal

Tactical advisor used mental rehearsal by imagining potential scenarios before choosing one when advising over the provision of a car

“Depending on the time and on the fuel switch, we could actually have the strike down within the cartilage of the tunnel area. Which gives us some control as they come out of the tunnel”

Anticipatory thinking

The IC visualized the consequences of their actions in terms of later stages of investigation, as well as post-incident procedures or examination of their decision-making and rationales

IC: So we are going to coroner’s court, and when we get to coroner’s court I need to be able to say that I got some advice and I was able to make a decision

IC: […] what I really need is some options so that I can start making choices. Because at the moment, she dies, other people die, I’m at coroner’s court or public enquiry and I am trying to explain some decisions I have made, and at the moment, I can’t make any. Because I’m not being given any options by my Tac Advisor, by my intelligence people, or by you

Weighing pros and cons

Plan prioritization

The IC and NC discussed whether or not to provide the hostage takers with a car

IC: In relation to the car, my gut instinct is to say no. However what this now presents us with is a tactical option. If we have more than one hostage on the bus, and we can present them with a car which means they take no hostages, I’ve saved a load of lives. And therefore presenting a car might actually give me some tactics

NC: So let the three go and then the car will be put in. What about… I appreciate what you were saying about not wanting to get into it, but are we possibly not letting them leave with two hostages?

IC: sorry, by all means give me another solution to save three lives. Because I can’t get into it, we can sit here forever and make them starve to death, or we can sit here forever and see if they just give people up. I mean, that is an option, but I have got to say, I’ve got no information on the identity of the hostages, I don’t know what illnesses they’ve got, I know that one has allegedly had her thumb cut off, so they are being physically assaulted now, so I actually have an imperative to do something. I can’t have a tactical imperative on that for all the reasons I have given you. So, that’s all I can come up with at the moment

Reduction (maladaptive PF)

Redundant deliberation

The IC sought advice on “known unknowns”; blocking the team decision process from reaching PE and instead directing them back to SA

“To that end, I don’t know if you’ve got there yet, or if they’ve asked what’s going to happen, it doesn’t sound like they’ve got any kind of conception that this is going to end any other way than the way they want it to. When it comes to reality, if they asked “what would happen if we gave ourselves up?” what would you say?”

Reduction-assumption-based reasoning loop (maladaptive PF)

Redundant deliberation

The IC and NC engaged in a Reduction-Assumption-based reasoning loop when redundantly deliberating whether to request the release of hostages

IC: “Now, your advice please on this—do we ask for all the hostages to be released or do we ask for one of the hostages to be released?” (reduction)

NC: “I don’t think it is reality that they would give up all the hostages because if they do give up all the hostages then they’ve given up bargaining chips. My view on these people is that the reason why they have the hostages is to buy them time, to mentally think about where they are and to escape.” (assumption-based reasoning)

IC: “How do the other hostages feel if we say “oh give up A hostage” […] that is the bit I need your advice on is—what is the best way to approach this? I want to save someone.” (reduction)

NC: “My advice to you is that I don’t think in the reality here that anybody will get seriously injured, if we play this strong.” (assumption-based reasoning)

Appendix 2.2: Example quotes of coping strategies coded during the PE phase

 

Coping strategy

Tactic used within strategy

Description of strategy in HN context

Example

Reduction

Using standard operating procedures

The IC, when deciding on the issue of implementing a communication device, sought advice from the technical support unit (TSU) officer and then implemented the decision based on the standard operating procedures for crisis negotiation

TSU: “AT the INK you’re only speaking on demand, whereas the field phone will have the dynamic footage coming in”

IC: “Perfect, on your advice that’s what we should do. Alright, I’m happier with that and I’ll tell you why I’m happier with that. Because it means that I now know that you are negotiating towards the planned objective, rather than endless “we’re just trying to chat” and that’s what we are meant to do. […]. Ok. Decision—you can go and tell them, they are going to negotiate a field phone in and then give them a reality check”

Assumption-based reasoning

Mental simulation

Officers would mentally simulate the exact procedures by which they would execute their actions and “play out” the anticipated consequences of that action

IC: “Right, ok, in order to help you get towards that option, I would like you (to NC) to get your team to negotiate in something, yeah? Because when I think the phone went in and we haven’t actually had any data back from that as to where he is and those other two are still on the bus. As soon as they are off the bus, I don’t care what it is, food or medicine, say let’s take food down there, yeah? Unconditional, he hasn’t asked for it, we’re worried about all of you, we’ve got some comfort for you.[…] we approach the bus with one, with a food parcel, leave it down for them, then we get an intelligence gain don’t we. Can you do me a favour, have a word with the TSU, explain what we are going to do, and whoever is going to take the food forward, we want them to have a camera on them and a recording device so that they can just talk through what they are seeing and what’s there and they can just give us a situation”

Forestalling

Preparing for worst-case scenario

The IC explicitly chose to prepare an option, but omit decision implementation; making it an available option if, in the “worst-case scenario”, they would have to implement it

“OK, right. Can you after this meeting go and do some work on that for me? and at least then I have got that as an option but at the moment I haven’t got any vehicle so I haven’t really got any tactical options at all. NC—what have you got for me?”

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

van den Heuvel, C., Alison, L. & Power, N. Coping with uncertainty: police strategies for resilient decision-making and action implementation. Cogn Tech Work 16, 25–45 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-012-0241-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10111-012-0241-8

Keywords

Navigation